J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;67:497–503
EVect of interstimulus interval on visual P300 inParkinson’s disease
Lihong Wang, Yoshiyuki Kuroiwa, Toshiaki Kamitani, Tatsuya Takahashi, Yume Suzuki,Osamu Hasegawa
Abstract
assessments, as they might be influenced by
Objective—Visual event related potentials
parkinsonian motor deficits during the task. (ERPs) were studied during an oddball
P300 latency, which is presumed to indicate
paradigm, to testify whether cognitive
the time required for task evaluation independ-
slowing in Parkinson’s disease exists and
ent of motor processing,6 may be useful in
to investigate whether cognitive infor- mation processing can be influenced by
disease. There are some reports which provide
diVerent interstimulus intervals (ISIs) of
evidence of cognitive slowing during auditory
an oddball task in patients with Parkin-
or visual oddball tasks by showing delayed
son’s disease and normal subjects.
P300 in Parkinson’s disease.7–11 However, there
Methods—ERPs and reaction time were
are also reports showing normal P300 latency
measured in 38 non-demented patients with Parkinson’s disease and 24 healthy
P300 is usually evoked by an oddball task
elderly subjects. A visual oddball para-
with an interstimulus interval (ISI) of 1000 ms
digm was employed to evoke ERPs, at
to 2000 ms. The load on cognitive processing
three diVerent interstimulus (ISI) inter-
systems varies with the rate at which infor-
vals: ISI(S), 1600 ms; ISI(M), 3100 ms; and
mation is presented—that is, with the duration
ISI(L), 5100 ms. The eVect of ISIs on ERPs
of the ISIs. A too fast or too slow stimulus
and reaction time was investigated.
presentation sometimes may make the task dif-
Results—Compared with the normal sub-
ficult to follow and to perform. Cognitive slow-
jects, P300 latency at Cz and Pz was
ing in Parkinson’s disease is presumed to be
significantly delayed after rare target
found only in tasks that place greater demands
stimuli in patients with Parkinson’s dis-
on the subject’s cognitive capacity.16 Therefore,
ease only at ISI(L). Reaction time was
we presumed that diVerent rates of information
prolonged in patients at all the three ISIs,
presentation due to diVerent ISIs of an oddball
compared controls.
task may change the diYculty of the task, and
There was also significantly delayed N200 and reduced P300 amplitude at Cz and/or
Parkinson’s disease. There have been few stud-
Pz to rare non-target stimuli in patients at
ies on the influences of diVerent presentation
the three ISIs, compared with the normal
rates on cognitive processing in patients with
controls. During rare target visual stimu-
Parkinson’s disease and normal subjects. lation, P300 latency and reaction time in
In this study, we intended to investigate
the patients with Parkinson’s disease and
whether the changes of visual event related
reaction time in the normal subjects were
potentials occur during rare target and rare
gradually prolonged as the ISI increased.
non-target stimulation when diVerent ISIs are
Department of Conclusion—Prolonged N200 latency to
used. To testify whether cognitive slowing in
Neurology, Yokohama rare non-target stimuli might indicate City University School
whether cognitive information processing can
that automatic cognitive processing was of Medicine, 3–9
be influenced by diVerent ISIs in patients with
Fukuura, slowed in Parkinson’s disease. Cognitive
Parkinson’s disease and normal subjects, visual
Kanazawa-ku, processing reflected by P300 latency to
ERPs and reaction time were measured during
Yokohama, 236–0004, rare target stimuli was influenced by
a modified oddball paradigm at short, medium,
longer ISI in patients with Parkinson’s
and long interstimulus intervals: ISI(S), 1600
disease.
ms; ISI(M),3100 ms; and ISI(L), 5100 ms.
(J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;67:497–503)
Keywords: P300; interstimulus interval; oddball para-
The subjects were 38 patients (14 men, 24
It has been a long debated question whether or
women) with the definite clinical diagnosis of
not there exists cognitive slowing in Parkin-
idiopathic Parkinson’s disease and 24 elderly
Yokohama City UniversitySchool of Medicine, 3–9
son’s disease.1–5 The neuropsychological tests
healthy volunteers (11 men, 13 women). The
employed to evaluate cognitive slowing are
age of the Parkinson’s disease group ranged
extremely varied. Until now, no consistent
from 41 to 77 years, with a mean (SD) of 65.8
Telephone 0081 45 7872800; fax 0081 45 788 6041.
conclusion has been reached on the existence
(8.8) years. The age of the normal control
and nature of the cognitive slowing in Parkin-
group ranged from 42 to 79 years, with a mean
son’s disease. It is sometimes diYcult to evalu-
(SD) of 65.2 (10.3) years. All control subjects
ate mental processing in patients with Parkin-
showed normal neurological findings and had
son’s disease accurately by neuropsychological
no specific neurological diseases. None exhib-
Backgrounds of patients with Parkinson’s disease
contained 12 to 16 responses to rare targetstimuli. Each section was repeated once to
confirm the reliability of recording for each ISI.
The EEG activity was analysed 100 ms before
and 900 ms after each visual presentation. On
the traces, upward deflection of the ERP wave-
nent (peak or notch) at Cz and Pz occurring
200–400 ms after the start of the stimulus.
P300 was identified as a positive wave at Cz
and Pz, with latency of 300–700 ms after the
start of the stimulus. N200 latency and P300
latency were measured as the interval between
each peak (or notch) and the onset of the
*The levodopa dosage in the table represents the dosage of
stimulus. P300 amplitude was defined as the
levodopa in Neodopaston® which contains levodopa and carbi-
voltage diVerence between the P300 peak and
the prestimulus baseline (averaging voltage of100 ms before the stimulus). Reaction time was
ited any abnormal MRI findings. They also had
defined as the interval between the start of rare
no history of medical or psychiatric disorders.
All patients fulfilled Parkinson’s Disease Soci-ety Brain Bank clinical criteria17 for definitedisease. Patients with secondary parkinsonism
or with evidence of focal cerebral lesions were
A two tailed Student’s t test was used to com-
excluded from the study. Any patients diag-
pare the ERPs (N200 latency, P300 latency,
nosed as having dementia according to the cri-
and P300 amplitude) or reaction time between
teria of the diagnostic and statistical manual of
the Parkinson’s disease and normal groups at
mental disorders, third edition, revised,18 were
each of the three ISIs. One way repeated meas-
excluded from the study. All patients were
ures ANOVA was computed to study the effect
of ISIs on ERPs (N200 latency, P300 latency,
Table 1 shows the details of the patients with
and P300 amplitude) or reaction time in the
Parkinson’s disease. All the subjects gave
Parkinson’s disease group and the normal con-
signed informed consent after the purpose of
trol group, separately. Two way repeated meas-
the study and the protocol had been explained
ures ANOVA was also studied using the groups
(Parkinson’s disease v normal controls) as the
between subjects factor and diVerent ISIs asthe within subjects factor. When ANOVAyielded significant diVerences among data sets,
a Bonferroni/Dunn test was performed as a
A modified visual oddball paradigm which was
previously designed by Tachibana et al19 wasused to elicit ERPs (fig 1). The paradigmemployed three kinds of visual stimuli: rare tar-get, rare non-target, and frequent non-target.
They were presented on an electronic tachisto-
All the normal subjects accurately performed
scope screen (Iwasaki Tsushin), at event prob-
the task. All patients with Parkinson’s disease
abilities of 20%, 20%, and 60%, respectively.
performed the task at an error rate not exceed-
The duration of each stimulus was 68 ms. The
ISI was defined as the interval between the
appearance of each sequential stimulus. The
Parkinson’s disease (fig 2), P300 latency seems
task was performed at short, medium, and long
to be longer after rare target stimuli at ISI(L)
ISI separately in the order of ISI(S) 1600 ms
than at ISI(S) and ISI(M). P300 amplitude
after rare non-target stimuli in patients with
(stimulus rate 0.322 Hz), and ISI(L) 5100 ms
Parkinson’s disease was markedly reduced
(stimulus rate 0.196 Hz). The subjects were
compared with that in normal controls at all
instructed to press the button for rare target
stimuli as rapidly and correctly as possible. Data were recorded only after the subjectsunderstood the tasks completely through a
trodes from Cz and Pz referred to linkedearlobes. The EOG was monitored using aforehead-temple montage with a rejection levelof ±100 µV. Electrode impedance was main-
tained below 5 K . Bandwidth of preamplifiers
ranged from 0.1 to 50 Hz. Each block included
four rare target stimuli. Three or four blocks
with breaks of 2 minutes between blocks com-
Sketch representing the time course of the
prised one section. Therefore, each section
modified oddball paradigm at three diVerent ISIs.EVect of interstimulus on P300 in Parkinson’s diseaseAveraged waveforms of ERPs at Pz after rare target and rare non-target at ISI(S), ISI(M), and ISI(L) of theParkinson’s disease (PD) and the normal control groups.
no ISI eVect was found on any of ERP compo-
BETWEEN PATIENTS WITH PARKINSON’S DISEASE
nents after rare target and rare non-target
stimuli. In the Parkinson’s disease group, only
We compared the data between patients with
P300 latency after rare target stimuli was found
Parkinson’s disease and normal control groups
to be significantly influenced by diVerent ISIs
using Student’s t test (tables 2 and 3). Prolonged P300 latency at Cz and Pz after rare
Comparison of the mean (SD) values of ERPs at
target stimuli in Parkinson’s disease was found
Pz and reaction time (RT) to rare target stimuli betweenpatients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) and normal controls
only at ISI(L) (Cz, t=2.408, df=52, p<0.05; Pz,
t=2.232, df=52, p<0.05). No significant N200latency or P300 amplitude diVerence to rare
target stimuli was found between groups at any
of the three ISIs. Significantly delayed N200 at
Parkinson’s disease was found at the three ISIs:
ISI(S),(Pz, t=2.915, df=49, p<0.01); ISI(M),
(Cz, t=2.269, df=50, p<0.05), and ISI(L),(Pz,
t=2.871, df=45, p<0.01). Significantly reduced
non-target stimuli was found in the Parkinson’s
disease group compared with the control group
at all the ISIs (ISI(S),Cz, t=2.741, df=58,
t=2.530, df=56, p<0.05; ISI(L), Cz, t=2.508,
*p<0.05; Student’s t test.
df=51, p<0.05; Pz, t=2.408, df=51, p<0.05). Comparison of the mean (SD) values of ERPs at
Reaction time to rare target stimuli in Parkin-
Pz to rare non-target stimuli between patients with
son’s disease was significantly delayed com-
Parkinson’s disease (PD) and normal controls at the three
pared with that in the control group at all the
ISIs (ISI(S), t=2.098, df=51, p<0.05; ISI(M),
t=2.475, df=49, p<0.05; ISI(L), t=2.483,df=48, p<0.05).
EFFECT OF ISI ON ERPS AND REACTION TIME IN
ANOVA to study the eVect of the three ISIs on
N200 and P300 components and reaction time
in the Parkinson’s disease and the normal
group, separately. In the normal control group,
*p<0.05; **p<0.01; Student’s t test. ISI eVect on reaction time in normal controlgroup (open circles) and Parkinson’s disease group (closedcircles). S, M, and L indicate ISI(S), ISI(M), and ISI(L),respectively. The p value represents the eVect of ISIs onreaction time by one way repeated measures ANOVA.NS=no significant diVerence among three ISIs by one way
stimuli (Cz, F=5.413, p<0.01; Pz, F=6.209,
p<0.01). P300 latency at ISI(L) was signifi-
cantly longer than at ISI(S) (Bonferroni/Dunn
post hoc, Cz, p<0.05; Pz, p<0.05). However,
ISI eVect on P300 latency at Cz and Pz in
groups×ISIs did not influence P300 latency to
normal control group(open circles) and Parkinson’s diseasegroup (closed circles). S, M, and L indicate ISI(S),ISI(M) and ISI(L), respectively. The p value represents the
groups made significant diVerences on N200
eVect of ISIs on P300 latency by one way repeated
latency (Cz, F=7.415, p<0.01; Pz, F=9.595,
measures ANOVA. NS=no significant diVerence among
p<0.01) and P300 amplitude (Cz, F=8.905,
three ISIs by one way repeated measures ANOVA.
p<0.01; Pz, F=8.220, p<0.01) to rare non-
F=8.094, p<0.001; Pz, F=5.006, p<0.05; fig 3
(Bonferroni/Dunn post hoc, Cz, p<0.01; Pz,
significantly longer than at ISI(S) (Bonferroni/
Dunn post hoc, Cz, p<0.05; Pz, p<0.05). No
(Bonferroni/Dunn post hoc, Cz, p<0.0001; Pz,
ISI eVect was found on any other ERP compo-
p<0.0001) to rare non-target stimuli compared
nents after rare target and rare non-target
with the control group. However, diVerent ISIs
stimuli in the Parkinson’s disease group.
or the interaction of groups×ISIs made no sig-
A significant ISI eVect on reaction time was
nificant diVerence on N200 latency and P300
found in both the Parkinson’s disease group
amplitude to rare non-target stimuli. DiVerent
groups, diVerent ISIs, and the interaction of
F=8.769, p<0.001; fig 4 and table 4) and the
groups×ISIs influenced reaction time to rare
normal control group (one way repeated meas-
target stimuli significantly (diVerent groups,
ures ANOVA, F=6.122, p<0.01). Reaction
F=6.441, p<0.05; diVerent ISIs, F=12.069,
time at ISI(L) was significantly longer than
p<0.0001; groups × ISIs, F=3.339, p<0.05).
reaction time at ISI(S) (Bonferroni/Dunn post
The patient group showed significantly more
hoc, p<0.05) in the Parkinson’s disease group
prolonged reaction time than the control group
and tended to be longer than reaction time at
(Bonferroni/Dunn post hoc, p<0.001). Reac-
tion time at ISI(L) was significantly more pro-
(Bonferroni/Dunn post hoc, p=0.0554).
(Bonferroni/Dunn post hoc, p<0.05). NeitherdiVerent groups nor diVerent ISIs had a
THE EFFECTS OF GROUPS AND ISIS ON ERPS AND
significant eVect on N200 latency and P300
REACTION TIMETwo way repeated measures ANOVA using the
amplitude to rare target stimuli, or P300
diVerent groups (Parkinson’s disease v normal
latency to rare non-target stimuli. Table 5 is a
controls) as the between groups factor and the
summary of the main significant findings in
diVerent ISIs as the within subjects factor con-
firmed that diVerent ISIs produced a signifi-cant P300 latency diVerence during rare target
MEASUREMENT OF P300 LATENCY DIFFERENCE(RATIO) AND REACTION TIME DIFFERENCE(RATIO) IN PATIENTS WITH PARKINSON’S DISEASE
Significant eVect of ISI on ERP and reactiontime (RT) to rare target stimuli in patients withParkinson’s disease (PD) and normal controls during one
As P300 latency and reaction time to rare tar-
get stimuli were found to be influenced by dif-ferent ISIs, we measured P300 latency diVer-
ence (or ratio) and reaction time diVerence (or
ratio) for two diVerent ISIs. P300 latency
Verences to rare target stimuli were defined
as (P300 latency at ISI(M)−P300 latency at
ISI(S)); (P300 latency at ISI(L)−P300 latency
at ISI(M)); and (P300 latency at ISI(L)−P300
*p<0.05, **p<0.01; ***p<0.001.
latency at ISI(S)). reaction time diVerences
EVect of interstimulus on P300 in Parkinson’s diseaseSignificant group and/or ISI eVect on ERPs and RT found during two way
elucidated why mean N200 latency in the Par-kinson’s disease group was delayed to rare
non-targets and was not delayed to rare targets.
Literature on visual search (popout) shows
that, even in normal subjects, verification of the
presence of a target is completed more quickly
than the decision that a target is absent.26 27
There is also a study showing that it was
significantly worse at discerning the orientation
of non-target regions than at detecting target
presence.28 These results might be related to a
self terminating processing theory that a visual
*p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001; ****p<0.0001.
search can terminate on the half way when a
predesignated target is found, while for a non-
ISI(M)−reaction time at ISI(S)); (reaction
target stimulus (a negative response), visual
time at ISI(L)−reaction time at ISI(M)); and
search must proceed until all features of the
(reaction time at ISI(L)− reaction time at
stimulus are evaluated.29 The significantly
ISI(S)). P300 latency ratios to rare target
delayed N200 to rare non-targets in our study
might be a physiological correlate to the self
ISI(M)/P300 latency at ISI(S)); (P300 latency
terminating processing, albeit exaggerated in
at ISI(L)/P300 latency at ISI(M)); and (P300
latency at ISI(L)/P300 latency at ISI(S)).
The controlled processing reflected by P300
Reaction time ratios were defined as (reaction
latency after rare targets was slowed only at
time at ISI(M)/reaction time at ISI(S)); (reac-
longer ISI in Parkinson’s disease, but not at
tion time at ISI(L)/reaction time at ISI(M));
and (reaction time at ISI(L)/reaction time at
that there was no group diVerence on P300
ISI(S)). We compared the above parameters
latency while taking all the three ISIs as a
between groups by Student’s t test. Only P300
whole. This result is diVerent from the results
of other authors who proved the cognitive
ISI(L)−P300 latency at ISI(M)) at Cz and
slowing during oddball task in Parkinson’s dis-
P300 latency ratio of (P300 latency at ISI(L)/
ease by showing delayed P300 latency,9–11 but in
P300 latency at ISI(M)) at Cz were signifi-
cantly greater in patients with Parkinson’s dis-
prolonged P300 latency in the disease.12–15 Dif-
ease than those in normal controls (latency
ferent clinical status such as age at test, age at
diVerence, t=−2.098, p<0.05; latency ratio,
onset, duration of illness, and treatment may be
able to explain such discrepancy on P300latency.30 31
Although the interaction of groups×ISIs did
Discussion
not influence P300 latency to rare target
stimuli significantly after two way repeated
reduced P300 amplitude at Cz and/or Pz to
rare non-target stimuli at all the three ISIs, and
that P300 latency to rare targets was signifi-
prolonged P300 latency at Cz and Pz to rare
cantly influenced by diVerent ISIs in the
targets only at ISI(L). Visual N200 to rare tar-
Parkinson’s disease group and not in the
normal control group. Furthermore, Student’s
auditory N2b20 and to be reflected an control-
t test on P300 to rare targets showed that (P300
led discrimination processing.21 However, our
visual N200 to rare non-target stimuli was
ISI(L)−P300 latency at ISI(M)), and (P300
more similar to N2b described by Squires et al,
latency at ISI(L)/P300 latency at ISI(M)) in
which appeared while the subject was paying
the Parkinson’s disease group showed signifi-
unconscious attention and should reflect an
cantly larger mean values than those in the
automatic task evaluation processing.22 P300
normal control group. All these results sug-
after rare non-target stimuli in our paradigm
gested that the ISI eVect on P300 to rare
was also thought to reflect an automatic task
targets was rather stronger in the Parkinson’s
evaluation processing,23 whereas P300 after
disease group compared with the normal con-
rare target stimuli could reflect controlled cog-
trol group. Polich et al32 described significantly
nitive processing.23 24 With regard to the
longer auditory P300 latency at the ISI of 6
question whether cognitive slowing exists in
seconds than the P300 latency at the ISI of 2
Parkinson’s disease or not, our finding of
seconds in normal younger subjects. Literature
prolonged N200 latency to rare non-targets
about ISI eVects on P300 latency using visual
implies that the automatic cognitive processing
stimulation in Parkinson’s disease is completely
could be slowed in the disease. Dysfunction of
lacking. To our knowledge, this is the first
automatic processing in Parkinson’s disease
report that showed P300 changes at diVerent
has also been reported by a recent ERP study.24
One possible explanation for delayed P300
studies25 which suggested preserved function of
at ISI(L) is that it might be due to the decreas-
automatic cognitive processing in parkinsonian
ing sustained attention at longer ISI in Parkin-
patients. Task diVerences while evaluating the
son’s disease. It is said that sustained attention
automatic cognitive processing may be a reason
is impaired in Parkinson’s disease.33 34 In the
longer ISI condition, the subjects had to try to
some slowness of automatic cognitive process-
maintain their attention for a relatively longer
ing in Parkinson’s disease, whereas controlled
period of time. Therefore, too slow information
cognitive processing was slowed only at the
input at ISI(L) might cause an eVort demand-
ing task to patients with Parkinson’s disease.
latency and reaction time implied that the con-
Further, to get the same number of trials per
trolled cognitive processing in Parkinson’s dis-
ISI condition, the total test session would be
ease was influenced by slower information
much longer in the longer ISI condition. Also,
the ISI(L) task was always performed later thanISI(S) and ISI(M). All these facts could result
We are grateful to Masaaki Sibuya, Professor of Statistics, Keio
in low attention at longer ISI. However, if it
University, Tokyo for helpful assistance and comments concern-ing statistical analysis. We also sincerely thank Ivan Bodis-
were a problem of low attention at longer ISI,
Wollner, Professor of Neurology and Ophthalmology, State
University of New York, for valuable comments on the paper.
longed P300 latency, should be more pro-nounced as P300 amplitude was thought to be
1 Rogers D. Bradyphrenia in parkinsonism: a historical review. Psychol Med 1986;16:257–65.
2 Lee AJ. The concept of bradyphrenia. Rev Neurol 1994;150:
capacity.35 In our study, P300 amplitude in
3 Pate DS, Margolin DI. Cognitive slowing in Parkinson’s
Parkinson’s disease was not influenced by
disease and Alzheimer’s patients: distinguishing bradyphre-
nia from dementia. Neurology 1994;44:669–74.
4 Russ MO, Seger L. The eVect of task complexity on reaction
times in memory scanning and visual discrimination in
Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 1995;33:561–75.
5 Spicer KB, Brown GG, Gorell JM. Lexical decision in Par-
kinson disease: lack of evidence for generalized bradyphre-
nia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1994;16:457–71.
6 McCarthy G, Donchin E. A metric for thought: a compari-
son of P300 latency and reaction time. Science 1981;211:
result in a failure of retaining iconic memory
7 Kutukcu Y, Marks WJ, Goodin DS, et al. Cerebral
on the previous stimulus in Parkinson’s dis-
accompaniments to simple and choice reaction tasks in
ease. However, we do not think that short term
Parkinson’s disease. Brain Res 1998;799:1–5.
8 Tachibana H, Aragane K, Miyata Y, et al. Electrophysiologi-
memory is crucial enough to produce a P300
cal analysis of cognitive slowing in Parkinson’s disease. J
diVerence among varying ISIs in our oddball
Neurol Sci 1997;149:47–56.
9 Hansch EC, Syndulko K, Cohen SN, et al. Cognition in
Parkinson disease: an event-related potential perspective. Ann Neurol 1982;11:599–607.
10 Prasher D, Findley L. Dopaminergic induced changes in
results, we assumed that the visual stimulus
cognitive and motor processing in Parkinson’s disease: an
input at ISI(S) was presented at a relatively fast
electrophysiological investigation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psy- chiatry 1991;54:603–9.
stimulus rate (0.625 Hz) and could give rise to
11 Elwan OH, Baradah OH, Madkour O, et al. Parkinson’s dis-
some sort of rhythmic visual sensation. On the
ease, cognition and aging. Clinical, neuropsychological,electrophysiological and cranial computerized topographic
other hand, the visual stimulus rate at ISI(L)
assessment. J Neurol Sci 1996;143:64–71.
was rather slow (0.196 Hz) and the rhythmic
12 Goodin DS, AminoV MJ. Electrophysiological diVerences
between demented and non-demented patients with
sensation would be relatively weaker. Clinical
Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol 1987;21:90–4.
findings and research showed that patients with
13 Stanzione P, Fattapposta F, Giunti P, et al. P300 variations
in parkinsonian patients before and during dopaminergic
Parkinson’s disease can gain an improvement
monotherapy: a suggested dopamine component in P300.
in gait disturbance by rhythmic auditory exter-
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1991;80:446–53.
14 Rumbach L, Tranchant C, Viel JF, et al. Event-related
nal cues37–42 In other words, typical gait distur-
potentials in Parkinson’s disease: a 12-month follow-up
bance in Parkinson’s disease can be seen with-
study. J Neurol Sci 1993;116:148–51.
15 Green J, Woodard JL, Sirockman BE, et al. Event-related
out rhythmic auditory stimuli. It has remained
potential P3 change in mild Parkinson’s disease. Mov Dis-
unknown whether or not mental processing in
ord 1996;11:32–42.
16 Brown RG, Marsden CD. Cognitive function in Parkinson’s
Parkinson’s disease might be facilitated by a
disease: from description to theory. Trends Neurosci
visual or auditory rhythmic information input.
1990;13:21–9.
17 Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, et al. Accuracy of clinical
Although the interpretation of our results
diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-
might be speculative, lack of such rhythmic
pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychia- try 1992;55:181–4.
visual sensation at ISI(L) could give rise to
18 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical
prolonged P300 latency in the Parkinson’s dis-
manual of mental disorders. 3rd ed, revised (DSM III-R). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1987.
19 Tachibana H, Toda K, Sugita M. Age-related changes in
attended and unattended P3 latency in normal subjects. Int J Neurol Sci 1992;66:277–84.
response at a fixed ISI might elucidate this.
20 Gaillard AWK, Verduin CJ. Comparisons across paradigms:
Although the normal controls showed longer
an ERP study. In: Posner MI, Marin OSM, eds. Attentionand performance XI. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New
reaction time at longer ISI, our one way and
two way ANOVA results showed that the ISI
21 Novak G, Ritter W, Vaughan HG Jr. The chronometry of
attention modulated processing and automatic mismatch
eVect was more prominent in Parkinson’s
detection. Psychophysiology 1992;29:412–30.
disease. Reaction time is the time from task
22 Squires NK, Squires KC, Hillyard SA. Two varieties of
long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable
presentation to button press. It is related to
auditory stimuli in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neuro-physiol 1975;38:387–401.
23 Tachibana H, Toda K, Sugita M. Actively and passively
cannot conclude that cognitive processing in
evoked P3 latency of event-related potentials in Parkinson’s
Parkinson’s disease is impaired at longer ISI
disease. J Neurol Sci 1992;111:134–42.
24 Lagopoulos J, Gordon E, Barhamali H, et al. Dysfunctions
with the reaction time result alone. However,
of automatic (P300a) and controlled (P300b) processing in
with the results of reaction time and P300
Parkinson’s disease. Neurol Res 1998;20:5–10.
25 Weingartner H, Burns S, Diebel R, et al. Cognitive
latency together, we may speculate that cogni-
impairments in Parkinson’s disease: distinguishing between
tive processing was influenced by diVerent
eVort-demanding and automatic cognitive processes. Psy-chiatr Res 1984;11:223–35.
26 Enns JT, Rensink RA. Influence of scene-based properties
on visual search. Science 1990;247:721–3.
27 Jolicoeur P. Orentation congruency eVects in visual search.
related potentials indicates that there may exist
Can J Psychol 1992;46:280–305. EVect of interstimulus on P300 in Parkinson’s disease
28 Scialfa CT, JoVe KM. Preferential processing of target
35 Kok A. Event-related-potential (ERP) reflections of mental
features in texture segmentation. Perception and Psychophys-
resources: a review and synthesis. Biological Psychologyics 1995;57:1201–8.
1997;45:19–56.
29 Zandt TV, Townsend JT. Self-terminating versus exhaustive
36 Barrett G, Starr A. Event-related potentials in disordered
processes in rapid visual and memory search: an evaluative
short-term memory in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neuro-
review. Perception and Psychophysics 1993;53:563–80. physiol 1987;(suppl 40):657–63.
30 Stanzione P, Semprini R, Pierantozzi M, et al. Age and stage
37 MaCarthy JA. Unusual phenomenon in Parkinson disease.
dependency of P300 latency alterations in non-demented
Ann Neurol 1979;5:499.
Parkinson’s disease patients without therapy. Electroen-
38 Enzensberger W, Fischer P-A. Metronome in Parkinson’s
cephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1998;108:80–91.
disease. Lancet 1996;347:1337.
31 Ebmeimer KP. A quantitative method for the assessment of
overall eVect from a number of similar electrophysiological
studies: description and application to event-related poten-
auditory-motor facilitation of gait patterns in patients with
tials in Parkinson’s disease. Electroencephalogr Clin Neuro-
Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1997;62: physiol 1992;84:440–6.
32 Polish J, Bondurant T. P300 sequence eVects probability
40 Morris M, Iansek R, Matyas T, et al. Abnormalities in the
and interstimulus interval. Physiol Behav 1997;61:843–9.
stride length-cadence relation in parkinsonian gait. Mov
33 Steinman SB, Steinman BA. Vision and attention. I:
Disord 1998;13:61–9.
Current models of visual attention. Optom Vis Sci 1998,75:
41 Nieuwboer A, Feys P, Weerdt W, et al. Is using a cue the clue
to the treatment of freezing in Parkinson’s disease?
34 Trepanier LL, Saint-Cyr JA, Lozano AM, et al. Neuro-
Physiotherapy Research International 1997;2:125–34.
psychological consequences of posteroventral pallidotomy
42 Thaut MH, McIntosh GC, Rice RR, et al. Rhythmic
auditory stimulation in gait training for Parkinson’s disease
1998;51:207–15.
patients. Mov Disord 1996;11:193–200.
FOR TOPICAL USE ON THE FACE ONLYWhat is the Most Important Information about ReFissa [Tretinoin Cream, USP (Emollient) 0.05%]?ReFissa is a serious medication. It does not eliminate wrinkles or repair sun-damaged skin. It may help treat fine wrinkles, spotty discoloration, and rough feeling skin, but it does not “cure” these conditions. ReFissa should only be used under supervision of your
STATE OF THE MUNICIPAL ADDRESS MASERUMULA PARK SPORT GROUND 06 MARCH 2009 10:00 By CLLR. Q.E. MOKHABELA MAYOR OF MAKHUDUTHAMAGA LOCAL MUNICIPALITY SOMA 2009 MAKHUDUTHAMAGA MUNICIPALITY Page 1 of 13 MADAM SPEAKER HOUNORABLE MEC OF LOCAL GOVERNMENT AND HOUSING, MAITE NKOANA-MASHABANE EXECUTIVE COMMITTEE MEMBERS CHIEF WHIP AND COUNCILORS TRADITIONAL LEADER