Prefrontal Cortex: The Present and the Future
In this chapter the current neuropsychological and
physiological evidence linking lateral and orbital
prefrontal cortex (PFCx) to human cognition and social
interchange will be reviewed in an attempt to provide a
summary of much of the work presented at the Rotman
Frontal Lobe meeting and delineated in this book. We
will begin with our view of the contributions of
lateral prefrontal cortex to executive control. This
book provides evidence of a remarkable convergence of
lesion, electrophysiological and fMRI data on the role
of lateral PFCx in inhibitory control, excitatory
modulation, working memory and novelty processing. The
electrophysiological data accrued from humans and
animals provide important information on the timing of
PFCx modulation of cognitive processing. This
electrophysiological data is complemented by fMRI
findings defining the spatial characteristics of PFCx
involvement in a variety of cognitive tasks with
evidence mounting for engagement of interleaved
inhibitory and excitatory processes during a host of
cognitive processes. Finally, the neuropsychological
data provides the crucial behavioral confirmation of
electrophysiological and functional magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI) findings obtained in normal populations.
In our view the most complete picture will emerge from
fusion of classic neuropsychological approaches
informed by cognitive theory with powerful new
techniques to measure human brain physiology. We first
review the findings concerning the role of lateral PFCx
in executive control of cognition and then discuss the
relevant literature on the contributions of orbital
PFCx to social and emotional control. This chapter will
not address the role of medial PFCx cortex in various
aspects of motor planning nor will we discuss language
and PFCx, which is reviewed by Alexander in this
volume. A possible separate role for the polar regions
may exist, but the evidence currently is inadequate to
Evidence from neuropsychological, electrophysiological
and neuroimaging research supports a critical role of
prefrontal cortex in executive control of goal directed
behavior. Lateral PFCx, including portions of the
inferior, middle and superior frontal gyri, is involved
in multiple domains including language, attention and
memory (Stuss & Benson, 1984, 1986; Corbetta et al.,
1998; D’Esposito et al., 1995, 1999a, b, c; Knight et
al., 1998; Chao & Knight, 1998; Dronkers et al., 2000;
McDonald et al., 2000; Fuster et al., 2000). Functional
MRI and event-related potential (ERP) research has
defined the spatial and temporal contributions of
lateral PFCx in working memory, attention, response
conflict and novelty processing (Jonides et al.,
1993,1998; Owen et al., 1998; Knight et al., 1998;
Botvinick et al., 1999; Barch et al., 2000; Prabhakaran
et al., 2000). A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies
(Duncan & Owen, 2000) reveals activation of common
regions of lateral PFCx in diverse cognitive tasks.
PFCx activations in these seemingly diverse cognitive
domains center in the posterior portions of the lateral
PFCx at the junction of the middle and inferior frontal
gyri including portions of dorsal and ventral PFCx
(Brodmann areas 9,44,45,46; Rajkowska & Goldman-Rakic,
1995a,b). This suggests that common regions of the
human prefrontal cortex are able to control many
aspects of human cognition. Further, fMRI research has
revealed that extensive areas of dorsal and ventral
PFCx are crucial for sustaining neural activations
during cognitive performance and more dorsal portions
of PFCx appear to be crucial for manipulating neural
activity during cognitive performance. Thus, a more
complete behavioral and physiological picture of the
functions of subregions of human lateral PFCx is
The extensive reciprocal PFCx connections to
virtually all cortical and subcortical structures
places PFCx in a unique neuroanatomical position to
monitor and manipulate diverse cognitive processes.
Lateral prefrontal damage in humans results in
behavioral deficits in attention, working memory,
planning, response selection, temporal coding,
metamemory, judgment and insight. In advanced
bilateral lateral PFCx damage the patient has a
flattened affect, manifests perseverative behavior and
is unable to properly manage everyday affairs. The
patient may be incorrectly diagnosed as being depressed
although careful evaluation actually reveals
indifference and an amotivational state rather than
true feelings of sadness This amotivational state can
be differentiated from the amotivation seen in patients
with pure superior medial, or orbitofrontal pathology
(Stuss, van Reekum, & Murphy, 2000). The amotivational
state extends to both the cognitive and emotional
domain. For instance, cognitive flexibility and
creativity as well as sexual drive are frequently
reduced. Frontal release signs (primitive reflexes
including snout, rooting, suck, grasp and palmomental)
are often observed (Knight & Grabowcky, 2000). The
Luria hand-sequencing task where the patient is
required to repetitively produce the sequence fist-palm
of hand-edge of hand is often abnormal due to
It should be emphasized that the severe lateral
prefrontal syndrome is typically observed in bilateral
PFCx damage as might be observed in degenerative
disease, infiltrating tumors or multiple cortical or
subcortical infarctions. This clinical observation
emphasizes the inherent redundancy in the capacity of
lateral PFCx function to control cognitive processing.
This is not to say that clear behavioral and
physiological deficits are not apparent after
unilateral damage. Rather, these deficits are not as
obvious as one might expect. One intact PFCx is able to
control bilateral hemispheric processing to some
degree. How this redundancy of function is accomplished
is not clear although recent animal work suggests that
processing may be accomplished by callosal transfer of
information to the intact PFCx (Rossi et al., 1999;
Tomita et al, 1999). The use of the intact processes in
the undamaged frontal lobe to overcome or compensate
for the deficit in the damaged region of the prefrontal
lobe has also been hypothesized (Stuss, Delgado &
Human behavior is paralleled by a massive evolution of
the prefrontal cortex, which occupies up to 35 percent
of the neocortical mantle in man. In contrast,
prefrontal cortex occupies about 10-12 percent of the
cortical mantle in high level non-human primates such
as gorillas (Fuster, 1989). Since lateral PFCx cortex
is involved in so many aspects of behavior,
characterization of a mild prefrontal syndrome can be
elusive. Prefrontal damage from strokes, tumors,
trauma or degenerative disorders is notoriously
difficult to diagnose since deficits in creativity and
mental flexibility may be the only salient findings
(Knight, 1991). The patient may complain that he is
not able to pay attention as well and that his memory
is not quite as sharp. An early lateral PFCx syndrome
may become clinically obvious only if the patient has a
job requiring some degree of mental flexibility and
decision making. However, if the patient has a
routinized job or lifestyle, prefrontal damage can be
quite advanced before a diagnosis is made. Indeed,
many prefrontal tumors are extensive at initial
diagnosis. As unilateral prefrontal disease progresses
or becomes bilateral pronounced abnormalities
invariably become evident. Deficits in attention,
planning, response selection, temporal coding,
metamemory, judgment and insight predominate. In
advanced bilateral lateral PFCx damage perseveration,
manifesting behaviorally as being fixed in the present
and unable to effectively go forward or backward in
time, becomes evident. In association with these
deficits, confidence about many aspects of behavior
deteriorates. Indeed, prefrontal patients may be
uncertain about the appropriateness of their behavior
The behavioral changes that arise from damage to
the prefrontal cortex are notoriously difficult to
capture with many standardized neuropsychological
tests. Patients with large prefrontal lesions can
perform within the normal range on tests of memory,
intelligence and other cognitive functions, one
observation that has led to the often-cited “paradox”
or “riddle” of the frontal lobes. Even supposedly
frontal lobe sensitive tests such as the Wisconsin Card
Sorting Test (WCST) sometimes fail to discriminate
patients with frontal lesions from normals or those
with lesions in other regions (Eslinger & Damasio,
1985; Grafman, Jonas, & Salazar, 1990). In part his is
due to a mixing of lesion location. Clinicians and
researchers often do not differentiate regions within
the frontal lobes. However, when one segregates
subregions of PFCx damage a clearer pattern of regional
specificity emerges. For instance, tests such as the
WCST appear to be more sensitive to dorsolateral than
orbitofrontal PFCx (see Stuss et al., this volume). In
addition, there is often a lack of specificity as to
the precise process that is impaired. For example, the
notion that only perseverative errors are made by PFCx
lesioned patients in the WCST may contribute to some of
the confusion in the literature. Recent findings in
patients with lateral PFCx damage indicate that these
patients make as many random errors as perseverative
errors (Barcelo & Knight, in press). Perseverative
errors are traditionally viewed as a failure in
inhibition of previous response pattern. A
perseverative error on the WCST is due to failure to
shift set to a new sorting criterion. Random errors
that are as frequent as perseverative errors reflect a
different problem in these patients. A random error
occurs when a patient is sorting correctly and switches
to a new incorrect sorting category without any prompt
from the examiner. Random errors can be viewed as a
transient failure in maintaining the goal at hand and
may represent a problem with maintenance of neural
activity during the sorting task. As will be discussed
later, excitation dependent maintenance of distributed
neural circuits appears to be a key aspect of PFCx
function (for reviews see Knight et al., 1998;
D’Esposito et al., 1999a). Other tasks often employed
by clinical neuropsychologists to capture lateral PFCx
deficits include variations of the Stroop test that is
particularly sensitive to failures in inhibitory
control and tests of divided attention. However, tests
of divided attention are also failed by patients with
inferior parietal lobe damage and do not provide a
specific assay of PFCx function. It should be
emphasized that patients with lesions in different
parts of the brain may fail the same task, but for
While conventional neuropsychological tests such
as the WAIS may be normal or only show minimal
deficits, patients with lateral PFCx cortex damage may
be quite impaired in their daily lives. How can this
paradox of relatively good performance on standardized
tests be reconciled with impaired functioning in daily
life? We will first review the current experimental
neuropsychological literature on lateral PFCx
dysfunction and then we will present some new concepts
drawn from the cognitive psychology literature that may
provide additional insight into understanding the human
lateral PFCx syndrome. After reviewing the
neuropsychological literature we will then discuss the
physiological data including ERPs and fMRI which
provide delineation of the role of lateral PFCx in
a. Temporal processing: Patients with frontal lobe
lesions are impaired in tasks involving temporal
ordering, such as the sequencing of recent or remote
events (McAndrews & Milner, 1991; Milner, Petrides, &
Smith, 1985; Moscovitch, 1989; Shimamura, Janowsky &
Squire, 1990). These patients are also impaired in
making recency judgments (Milner, 1971; Janowsky,
Shimamura, & Squire, 1989a; 1989b; Milner, Petrides, &
Smith, 1985), a process that also relies on the correct
temporal coding of events. Self ordered pointing, a
task in which the patient must remember the order in
which objects have been indicated, is also impaired in
patients with frontal lesions (Petrides & Milner,
1982). Patients with extensive frontal lesions also
have little concern for either the past or for the
future (Ackerly & Benton, 1947; Goldstein, 1944).
These patients are “stuck” in the present world with
severe perseveration representing the penultimate
example of failure to move across the time dimension.
Note that to fluidly move from the present to either
the past or the future one must be able to both inhibit
the current mental context and find or construct a new
mental image which requires excitation dependent
activation of neural ensembles as will become apparent
when physiological data relevant to PFCx function are
b. Explicit memory, source memory and metamemory: While
some sources suggest that explicit memory is normal in
patients with lateral PFCx damage, a meta-analysis of
the published data reveals significant deficits in
explicit memory in lateral PFCx patients (Wheeler,
Stuss & Tulving, 1995). These deficits are typically
not as severe a patients with medial temporal amnesia
but this distinction is compromised by the fact that
most studies have compared unilateral PFCx lesions to
bilateral medial temporal amnestics such as that
occurring from CA1 hypoxic damage or Herpes Simplex.
In other studies patient with Korsakoff’s syndrome or
ruptured anterior communicating aneurysms have been
compared to unilateral PFCx patients. Thus, the true
degree of explicit memory dysfunction in patients with
bilateral PFCx damage has been probably underestimated.
If the pathology is in the left PFCx or involves
septal/basal-forebrain areas, an encoding deficit is
revealed (Stuss et al., 1994). Patients with damage to
prefrontal cortex show a disproportionate impairment in
the memory for the source of information (Schacter,
Harbluk, & McLachlan, 1984; Shimamura & Squire, 1987;
Janowsky, Shimamura, & Squire, 1989b). Factual
information is correctly recalled but the spatio-
temporal context in which the information was acquired
is forgotten. These patients also have a diminished
ability to make metamemory judgments (Janowsky,
Shimamura & Squire, 1989b). Metamemory includes an
ability to judge whether or not the answer to a factual
question has been or will be correctly retrieved.
Patients with frontal lesions are impaired at making
these judgments even though their memory for the facts
is intact. In contrast, patients with medial temporal
amnesia have explicit memory impairments but are quite
confident about the limited number of items they
c. Inhibitory control: There is long standing evidence
that distraction due to a failure in inhibitory control
is a key element of the deficit observed in monkeys on
delayed response tasks (Malmo, 1942; Brutkowski, 1965;
Bartus & Levere, 1977). For example, simple maneuvers
such as turning off the lights in the laboratory or
mildly sedating the animal, which would typically
impair performance in intact animals, improved delay
performance in animals with PFC lesions. Despite this
evidence, remarkably little data has been obtained in
humans with PFCx damage. The extant data centers on
failures in inhibition of early sensory input as well
as problems in inhibition of higher level cognitive
In the sensory domain, it has been shown that
inability to suppress irrelevant information is
associated with difficulties in sustained attention,
target detection and match-to-sample paradigms in both
monkeys and humans (Woods & Knight 1986; Richer et al.,
1993; Chao & Knight 1995; 1998). Delivery of task
irrelevant sensory information disproportionately
reduces performance in patients with lateral PFC
lesions. For example, presentation of brief high
frequency tone pips during a tone matching delay task
markedly reduces performance in PFCx patients. In
essence, the patient with a lateral PFCx lesion
functions in a noisy environment due to a failure in
gating out extraneous sensory information.
In the cognitive domain, inhibitory deficits in
cognitive tasks requiring suppression of prior learned
material are also observed in patients with lateral
PFCx lesions (Shimamura et al., 1995; Mangels et al.,
1996). Prior learned information now irrelevant to the
task intrudes on performance. For example, words from a
prior list of stimuli employed in a memory task may be
inappropriately recalled during recall of a subsequent
list of words. In essence, the PFC patient is unable
to sweep the internal mental slate clean resulting in
previously learned material maintaining an active
neural representation. Inability to suppress previous
incorrect responses may underlie the poor performance
of PFCx subjects on a wide range of neuropsychological
tasks such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task and on
Stroop Task (Shimamura et al., 1992). Interestingly,
there is some evidence that inhibitory failure extends
to some aspects of motoric control. For instance,
lateral PFC damage results in a deficit in suppressing
reflexive eye movements to task irrelevant spatial
d. Working memory/attention: Working memory and
attention are core concepts necessary to understand
lateral PFCx function. Working memory refers to the
ability to maintain information over a delay and to
manipulate the contents of this short-term memory
storage system. Working memory is ubiquitous to many
cognitive tasks. A trivial example of working memory
would be remembering a phone number just obtained from
the operator. This task would only require maintenance
of the numbers over a few second period and would
typically not be impaired in unilateral disease.
However, if one were asked to remember the same number
over the same few second delay but now also respond at
the end of the delay as to whether this number matched
a number given a few minutes previously, deficits would
emerge in patients with lateral PFCx disease. In the
second situation, both maintenance and manipulation of
the contents of working memory are required and both of
these are dependent on lateral PFCx. Experimental
findings provide a critical link between the animal and
human working memory literature. Monkeys with bilateral
frontal lesions involving the sulcus principalis,
proposed to be equivalent to human lateral PFCx
(Brodmann areas 9 and 46: Rajkowska & Goldman-Rakic,
1995a,b), are severely impaired at delayed response
tasks (Jacobsen, 1935). In delayed response tasks,
information critical to perform a certain task is
initially presented. The experimenter then interposes
a delay period before the animal or human is allowed to
perform the task. For successful performance, the
information must be reliably held in a short-term
working memory buffer during the delay period.
Ablation, cryogenic depression or dopamine depletion in
the sulcus principalis area results in an inability for
the monkey to retain the critical information at
intervals as short as one second (Funahashi et al.,
Subsequent animal research revealed that problems
with inhibition of extraneous inputs contributed to the
delayed response deficit. Simple maneuvers such as
turning off the lights in the laboratory or mildly
sedating the animal, which would typically impair
performance in intact animals, improved delay
performance in prefrontal lesioned animals. These
observations led to the formulation of the
distractibility hypothesis (Malmo, 1942; Bartus &
Levere, 1977). This postulates that prefrontal
patients are unable to suppress responses to irrelevant
stimuli during delay tasks. Impairments in inhibitory
control in prefrontal patients and fMRI evidence
linking lateral PFCx to inhibitory control provides
further support for the prefrontal-distractibility
hypothesis. Successful performance on the delayed
response task of course requires more than inhibitory
control. Subjects must select and activate distributed
brain regions depending on task specific parameters.
Data from neurological patients has revealed that
lateral PFCx modulates excitatory pathways projecting
into subregions of visual and auditory association
cortices during attention and working memory tasks.
In accord with these physiological deficits in
inhibition and excitation, prefrontal patients are
distractible and unable to maintain the focus of
Working memory tasks in humans, widely viewed as
dependent on lateral PFCx, share a core task structure
with the monkey delayed response task. Single unit,
lesion, ERP, blood flow and neural modelling (Funahashi
et al., 1993, Chao & Knight, 1998, Jonides et al.,
1993, Cohen et al., 1996, Rainer et al., 1998a,b) have
shown that lateral PFCX is required to perform any task
requiring a delay. Thus, delayed response tasks and
working memory share some common neural mechanisms in
animals and man. Despite the clear link between working
memory, delayed response performance and PFCx there
have been a limited number of experimental studies on
working memory capacity in humans with PFCx lesions.
Moreover, working memory tasks are infrequently
employed in clinical neuropsychological work. However,
all studies examining working memory in patients with
lateral PFCx lesions have reported deficits (Chao &
Knight, 1995, 1998; Stone et al., 1998; Harrington et
Prefrontal patients are also impaired in their
ability to focus attention on task-relevant stimuli
(Knight et al., 1981; Damasio, 1985; Woods & Knight,
1986). It should be noted that attention deficits are
often more severe after right PFCx damage. Right
prefrontal patients show electrophysiological and
behavioral evidence of a dense hemi-inattention to left
ear stimuli (Woods & Knight 1986) in accord with the
human hemi-neglect syndrome which is more common after
right prefrontal or temporal-parietal lesions (Kertesz
& Dobrolowski, 1981; Mesulam, 1981, 1998). Increased
size of the right frontal lobe in humans may provide
the anatomical basis for the hemi-inattention syndrome
in humans. In this view the left hemi-neglect
syndrome subsequent to right temporal-parietal damage
may be due to remote effects of disconnection from
asymmetrically organized prefrontal regions.
e. Novelty processing: The capacity to detect novelty
in the stream of external sensory events or internal
thoughts and the ability to produce novel behaviors is
crucial for new learning, creativity and flexible
adjustments to perturbations in the environment.
Indeed, creative behavior in fields extending from
science to the arts is commonly defined in direct
relation to the degree of novelty. Prefrontal patients
have problems with the solving of novel problems
(Godfrey & Rousseaux, 1997; Goldberg et al., 1994) and
the generation of novel behaviors (Daffner et al.,
2000a,b). In advanced disease indifference, loss of
creativity and deficits in orienting to novel stimuli
emerge. In accord with these clinical observations,
prefrontal damage results markedly reduces the scalp
electrophysiological response to unexpected novel
stimuli in the auditory (Knight, 1984; Knight &
Scabini, 1998), visual (Knight 1997) and somatosensory
modalities (Yamaguchi & Knight, 1991b; Yamaguchi &
Knight, 1992). The physiological link between PFCx and
novelty processing will be expanded in the physiology
f. Behavioral monitoring: Behavioral output is
constantly monitored so the incorrect responses can be
detected and corrected. One notices this system in
operation in everyday behavior such as reaching for the
wrong object on a table and attempting to on-line
correct the incorrect movement. Another example might
be attempting to stop a swing at a baseball pitch that
is not in the strike zone. One might extend this notion
to the monitoring of complex cognitive and social
exchange. For instance, two linked classes of higher
level cognitive operations referred to as simulation
behavior and reality checking are proposed to be
impaired after lateral PFCx damage (Knight &
Grabowecky, 2000). Simulation refers to the process of
generating internal models of external reality. These
models may represent an accurate past or an alternative
past, present, or future and include models of the
environment, of other people, and of the self. Reality
checking refers to processes that monitor information
accrued from interactions with the external world in an
effort to accurately represent their spatio-temporal
context. These monitoring processes are critical for
discriminating between simulations of alternate
possibilities and veridical models of the world.
Simulation and reality checking are considered
"supervisory" (Shallice, 1988), or "executive"
(Baddeley & Wilson, 1988; Milner & Petrides, 1984;
Stuss & Benson, 1986) and are essential for behavior to
be integrated, coherent, and contextually appropriate.
Simulation behavior and reality checking are necessary
for permitting actions to be dissociated from current
environmental constraints. They permit humans to create
mental representations of the world that may either
draw on prior experience be entirely innovative. A
patient who cannot simulate alternatives to a situation
becomes "stimulus bound" (Luria, 1966; Lhermitte, 1986;
Lhermitte et al., 1986) and is incapable of responding
flexibly. Without reality checking, a patient cannot
discriminate between internally generated possibilities
and the model of the external world as it currently
exists. Simulation and reality checking work in
concert, allowing humans to simulate manipulations of
the external environment, evaluate the consequences of
those manipulations, and act on the results of those
Stimulus bound behavior is typical in PFCx
patients (Luria, 1966; Lhermitte, 1986). The patients
studied by Lhermitte and colleagues included large
lateral PFCx lesions that extended into the orbital and
basal ganglia regions in some subjects. Thus, precise
behavioral-anatomical conclusions must be tempered.
Objects placed in front of prefrontal patients in the
Lhermitte studies are picked up and used (utilization
behavior) without the patient being asked to do so
(Lhermitte, 1986). Behavior of the experimenter may be
imitated, even when this behavior is bizarre and
socially inappropriate. Thus, patients with frontal
lesions appear excessively bound by environmental cues.
Patients with prefrontal cortex lesions have also been
described as lacking insight and foresight, as
incapable of planning either for the near or distant
future, and as deficient in creativity (Ackerly &
Benton, 1948; Eslinger & Damasio, 1984; Hebb &
Penfield, 1940; Damasio, 1985). This set of abnormal
behaviors may be a consequence of a deficit in the
ability to simulate alternative scenarios of the
current situation. Once again precise behavioral-
anatomical conclusion cannot be obtained since clear
lesion definition is only provided in the Damasio
cohort with predominantly orbital damage.
The term "reality checking" refers to those
aspects of monitoring the external world that have been
called "reality testing" when they concern the present,
and "reality monitoring" when they concern the past.
Reality checking includes both an awareness of the
difference between an internally generated alternate
reality and a current reality, and the maintenance of a
true past in the presence of counterfactual
alternatives that one might construct. Reality
checking is essential for simulation processes to be
carried out without compromising the ability to respond
to the objective environment. Simulation processes
generate an alternate reality that must be evaluated in
relation to its divergence from the current reality.
Memories are created for both events experienced in the
world and events experienced through internally
constructed simulations. These two sources of memories
must be treated differently in order for them to be
used effectively. Given that both internal and
external events create memory representations, what
cues differentiate our internal models of reality from
our internal simulations of reality? Johnson and Raye
(1981) studied normal subjects abilities to
discriminate between memories of external events and of
internally generated events. Rich memory traces with
many sensory features were ascribed to external
experience. Memories of external events tend to be
more detailed and have more spatial and temporal
contextual information. Internally generated memories
tend to be abstract and schematic, lacking in detail.
These two memory representations form overlapping
populations, and similar internal and external events
may become confused. Reality checking involves a
continual assessment of the relationship between
behavior and the environment. As an individual acts on
the environment, the consequences of the action must be
incorporated into existing plans. If the environment
deviates from expectations, one needs to detect this
change and plans must be reassessed. It is proposed
that these processes of continual reality checking and
simulation are impaired in patients with frontal lobe
lesions (Knight & Grabowecky, 2000). There is a
paucity of neuropsychological literature linking
simulation and reality monitoring to PFCx, although
some data indicates that monitoring in memory is
related to dorsolateral areas, more on the right (Stuss
et al., 1994). Reality monitoring has also been
proposed as a major mechanism underlying different
disturbances of self-awareness (Stuss, 1991; Stuss et
Physiology research strongly supports, and extends, the
results demonstrated in the neuropsychological
literature. The physiological literature demonstrates
the temporal unfolding of frontal lobe processes. In
this section, we will emphasize that basic concepts
such as inhibitory and excitatory control, a bias to
novelty and response monitoring can provide useful
physiological constructs to begin to understand PFCx
a. Inhibitory control: PFCx inhibitory control of
subcortical (Edinger et al., 1975) and cortical regions
has been documented in a variety of mammalian
preparations (Alexander et al., 1976; Skinner &
Yingling, 1977; Yingling & Skinner, 1977). Galambos
(1956) provided the first physiological evidence of an
inhibitory auditory pathway in mammals with the
description of the brainstem olivo-cochlear bundle. The
olivo-cochlear bundle projects from the olivary nucleus
in the brainstem to the cochleus in the inner ear.
Stimulation of this bundle results in inhibition of
transmission from the cochlea to the brainstem cochlear
nucleus as measured by reductions in evoked responses
in the auditory nerve. This pathway provides a system
for early sensory suppression in the auditory system.
The evidence for sensory filtering at the cochlear or
brainstem level in humans is controversial, with most
laboratories finding no evidence of attention-related
manipulation of the brainstem auditory evoked response
(Woods & Hillyard, 1978; Woldorff & Hillyard, 1991).
Subsequent research in the 1970’s reported
evidence of a multi-modal prefrontal-thalamic
inhibitory system in cats that regulates sensory flow
to primary cortical regions. Reversible suppression of
the cat PFC by cooling (cryogenic blockade) increased
the amplitudes of evoked responses recorded in primary
cortex in all sensory modalities (Skinner & Yingling,
1977; Yingling & Skinner, 1977). Conversely,
stimulation of the thalamic region (nucleus reticularis
thalami) surrounding the sensory relay nuclei resulted
in modality specific suppression of activity in primary
sensory cortex. This effect is also observed in all
sensory modalities. These data provided the first
physiological evidence of a prefrontal inhibitory
pathway regulating sensory transmission through
thalamic relay nuclei. This prefrontal-thalamic
inhibitory system provides a mechanism for modality
specific suppression of irrelevant inputs at an early
stage of sensory processing. As noted, this system is
modulated by an excitatory lateral PFCx projection to
the nucleus reticularis thalami, although the precise
course of anatomical projections between these
structures is not well understood. The nucleus
reticularis thalami in turn sends inhibitory GABA-ergic
projections to sensory relay nuclei, providing a neural
substrate for selective sensory suppression (Guillery
There is also evidence in humans that the PFCx
exhibits control on other cortical and subcortical
regions. For example, ERP studies in patients with
focal PFCx damage has shown that primary auditory and
somatosensory evoked responses are enhanced (Knight et
al., 1989a; Yamaguchi & Knight, 1990; Chao & Knight,
1998) suggesting disinhibition of sensory flow to
primary cortical regions. In a series of experiments,
task irrelevant auditory and somatosensory stimuli
(monaural clicks or brief electric shocks to the median
nerve) were presented to patients with comparably sized
lesions in lateral PFCx, the temporal-parietal
junction, or lateral parietal cortex. Evoked responses
from primary auditory (Kraus et al., 1982) and
somatosensory (Leuders et al., 1983; Sutherling et al.,
1988; Wood et al., 1988) cortices were recorded from
these patients and age-matched controls. Damage to
primary auditory or somatosensory cortex in the
temporal-parietal lesion group reduced the early
latency (20-40 msec) evoked responses generated in
these primary cortical regions. Posterior association
cortex lesions in the lateral parietal lobe sparing
primary sensory regions had no effect on early sensory
potentials and served as a brain-lesioned control
group. Lateral PFC damage resulted in enhanced
amplitudes of both the primary auditory and
somatosensory evoked responses (Knight et al., 1989a;
Yamaguchi & Knight, 1990; Chao & Knight, 1998). Spinal
cord and brainstem potentials were not affected by
lateral PFC damage, suggesting that the amplitude
enhancements were due to abnormalities in either a
prefrontal-thalamic or a prefrontal-sensory cortex
mechanism. These results are in accord with the
findings reported in the 70’s by Yingling and Skinner
in their cat model of PFC dependent sensory gating.
Behavioral and imaging evidence of the involvement
of lateral PFC in inhibitory control does not provide
direct support for the hypothesis that there are
inhibitory signals from PFC directed either toward
early sensory cortices or excitatory PFC inputs to the
Gabaergic nucleus reticularis thalami resulting in a
net inhibitory control of sensory flow. In contrast,
the combined ERP/patient studies as described are able
to measure the temporal dynamics of inhibitory control
and provides powerful evidence in humans that the PFC
provides a net inhibitory regulation of early sensory
b. Excitatory control: Attention allows us select from
the myriad of closely spaced and timed environmental
events. Attention is crucial for virtually all
cognitive abilities. Indeed, recent cognitive theorists
have begun to refer to attention/working memory
highlighting that these two constructs are inextricably
linked. In addition to suppressing response to
irrelevant stimuli, subjects must excite and sustain
neural activity in distributed brain regions in order
to perform attention/working memory tasks. Neural
modelling employing prefrontal excitatory modulation of
distributed brain regions has successfully modeled
prefrontally mediated behaviors in normals and
prefrontal dysfunction in schizophrenia (Cohen &
Servan-Schrieber, 1992; Cohen et al., 1996). These
authors postulate that dorsal PFCx controls task
context by regulating posterior association cortex
through excitatory connections. Desimone (1998) has
proposed a competition based model of visual attention
wherein visual neurons involved in processing of
different aspects of the visual world are mutually
inhibitory. In this view an excitatory signal to
selective visual neurons would result in inhibition of
nearby non-task relevant visual neurons resulting in a
sharpening of the attentional focus. Patients with
focal prefrontal damage fail to maintain excitatory
control of posterior association cortex resulting in
Selective attention to an ear, a region of the
visual field or a digit increases the amplitude of
sensory evoked potentials to all stimuli delivered to
that sensory channel (Hillyard et al., 1973). There is
evidence that attention reliably modulates neural
activity at early sensory cortices including secondary
and perhaps primary sensory cortex (Woldorff et al.,
1993; Grady et al., 1997; Somers et al., 1999;
Steinmetz et al., 2000). Visual attention involves
modulation in the excitability of extrastriate neurons
through descending projections from hierarchically
ordered brain structures (Hillyard & Anllo-Vento,
1998). Single cell recordings in monkeys (Fuster et
al., 2000; Funahashi et al., 1993; Rainer et al.,
1998a,b), lesion studies in humans (Knight, 1997;
Nielsen-Bohlman & Knight, 1999; Knight et al., 1998;
Barcelo et al., 2000) and monkeys (Rossi et al., 1999)
and blood flow data (McIntosh et al., 1994; Buchel &
Friston, 1997; Chawla et al., 1999; Rees et al., 1997;
Kastner et al., 1999; Corbetta et al., 1998; Hopfinger
et al., 2000) have linked PFCx to control of
extrastriate cortex during visual attention.
Modulation of visual pathway activity has been
extensively investigated in humans using event-related
potentials (ERPs). Attended visual stimuli evoke
distinct ERP signatures. Attention enhances
extrastriate ERP amplitudes for all stimuli in an
attended channel with changes apparent in the initial
100-200 milliseconds after delivery of a to be attended
visual stimulus (Heinze et al., 1994; Mangun, 1995;
Martinez et al., 1999; Woldorff et al., 1997). These
early human ERP components have been linked to
increased firing of extrastriate neurons in monkeys
(Luck et al., 1997) providing a powerful parallel
between the human and animal literature.
From ERP studies in patients with lateral PFC
damage, evidence has accumulated that human lateral PFC
regulates attention dependent extrastriate neural
activity through three distinct mechanisms. These
mechanisms include: (1) an attention dependent
enhancement of extrastriate cortex, (2) a tonic
excitatory influence on ipsilateral posterior areas for
all sensory information including attended and non-
attended sensory inputs and (3) a phasic excitatory
influence of ipsilateral posterior areas to correctly
perceived task relevant stimuli. In these ERP studies,
patients with unilateral PFC lesions (centered in
Brodmann’s areas 9 and 46) performed a series of visual
attention experiments. In the task, non-target stimuli
consisted of upright triangles, which were presented
rapidly to both visual fields (4 degrees from the
fovea). Targets were rarely presented (10% of all
stimuli) and consisted of inverted triangles presented
randomly in each visual field. In one experiment,
patients and age-matched controls were asked to press a
button whenever a target appeared in either visual
field (Barcelo et al., 2000). In another experiment,
subjects were required to allocate attention to only
one visual field (Yago & Knight, 2000).
An interesting pattern of results emerged from
these two experiments. First, both experiments revealed
that lateral PFC provides a tonic excitatory influence
to ipsilateral extrastriate cortex. Specifically, the
P1 component of the visual ERP is markedly reduced in
amplitude for all stimuli presented to the
contralesional field. Importantly, this tonic
influence is attention independent since a reduced P1
potential in extrastraite cortex was found ipsilateral
to PFC damage for all visual stimuli (attended and non-
attended targets and non-targets) presented to the
contralesional field This tonic component may be viewed
as a modulatory influence on extrastriate activity.
As noted previously, it is well known that
attention increases the amplitude of extrastriate ERPs
in normals with effects onsetting by about 50-100
milliseconds post stimulus delivery. The second
experiment (allocating attention to only one visual
field) provided evidence of the temporal kinetics of
prefrontal-extrastriate interactions. In essence,
attention effects on extrastriate cortex were normal in
the first 200 milliseconds of processing in PFCx
patients and severely disrupted after 200 milliseconds
(Yago & Knight, 2000). This finding suggests that other
cortical areas are responsible for attention dependent
regulation of extrastriate cortex in the first 200
milliseconds. A candidate structure for this influence
based on the neuroimaging and clinical literature would
be inferior parietal cortex. It is conceivable that
inferior parietal cortex is responsible for the early
reflexive component of attention whereas PFC is
responsible for more controlled and sustained aspects
of visual attention onsetting after the parietal signal
The third observation from these experiments is
the finding that lateral PFC has been shown to send a
top-down signal to extrastriate cortex when a task
relevant event is detected during an attention task.
There are two types of stimuli typically presented in
an attended channel, one task irrelevant and one
requiring detection and a behavioral response. The
amplitude of both the irrelevant and relevant stimuli
is enhanced in an attended channel. As discussed
previously, PFCx is responsible for regulating this
channel specific attention enhancement. When a relevant
target event is detected in an attended channel another
distinct electrophysiological event is generated in
addition to the channel specific enhancement. This top-
down signal onsets at about 200 milliseconds after a
correct detection, extends throughout the ensuing 500
milliseconds and is superimposed on the channel
specific ERP attention enhancement (Suwazono et al.,
2000). Damage to lateral PFCx results in marked
decrements in the top-down signal accompanied by
behavioral evidence of impaired detection ability
The temporal parameters of this human PFCx-
extrastriate attention modulation are in accord with
single unit recordings in monkeys that reveal enhanced
prefrontal stimulus detection-related activity 140 ms
post-stimulus onset (Rainer et al., 1998a,b) and other
studies revealing top-down activation of inferior
temporal neurons 180-300 ms post-target detection
(Tomita et al., 1999). Finally, there is a vigorous
debate in the single unit and fMRI research domains on
the whether lateral PFCx is organized by modality
(Wilson & Goldman-Rakic, 1993; Courtney et al., 1998;
Romanski et al., 1999) or whether lateral PFCx, and
more particularly dorsolateral PFCx, functions in a
modality independent executive manner during working
memory and object and spatial integration (Rao et al.,
1997; Assad et al., 1998; D’Esposito et al., 1999b;
Miller, 1999; Fuster et al., 2000). Evidence from PFCx
lesioned patients (Mulleret al,, in press) supports the
notion that the lateral portion of PFCx may function in
a task independent manner to control and integrate
distributed neural activity in some cognitive tasks.
Projections from prefrontal areas 45 and 8 to
inferior temporal (IT) areas TE and TEO have been
demonstrated in monkeys (Webster et al., 1994)
providing a possible glutamatergic pathway by which
lateral prefrontal cortex could facilitate visual
processing. A similar failure of prefrontal excitatory
modulation is observed in the auditory modality.
Prefrontal lesions markedly reduce the attention
sensitive N100 component throughout the hemisphere
ipsilateral to damage (Chao & Knight, 1998). There are
well-described prefrontal projections to the superior
temporal plane, which may subserve this excitatory
PFCx-auditory cortex input (Alexander et al., 1976).
The auditory and visual data provide clear evidence
that lateral PFCx cortex is crucial for maintaining
distributed intrahemispheric neural activity during
auditory and visual attention/working memory tasks.
c. Novelty processing: The neural mechanisms of novelty
detection and the production of novel behavior are
receiving increasing attention. Multiple experimental
approaches have focussed on the biological mechanisms
electrophysiological data have shown that novel events
are better remembered (Von Restorff, 1933; Karis et
al., 1984). On a molecular basis, genetic studies of
novelty seeking behavior in humans have provided a link
to the short arm of chromosome 11 and the dopamine D4
receptor gene (Benjamin et al., 1996; Ebstein et al.,
1996). Integrative neuroscience approaches including
neuropsychological, electrophysiological and cerebral
blood flow techniques have revealed that a distributed
neural network including lateral PFCx, temporal-
parietal junction, hippocampus and cingulate cortex is
engaged both by novelty detection and during the
Studies in normals have shown that novel items generate
a late-positive ERP peaking in amplitude at about 300-
500 milliseconds that is maximal over the anterior
scalp. This novelty ERP is proposed to be a central
marker of the orienting response (Sokolov, 1963;
Courchesne et al., 1975; Knight, 1984; Yamaguchi &
Knight, 1991; Bahramali et al., 1997; Escera et al.,
1998). ERP evidence derived from neurological patients
with lateral PFCx damage (Yamaguchi & Knight,
1991,1992; Verleger et al., 1994; Knight, 1996; Knight,
1997; Knight et al., 1989b) and intracranial ERP
recordings in pre-surgical epileptics (Halgren et al.,
1998) has revealed that a distributed neural network
including lateral and orbital PFCx, hippocampal
formation, anterior cingulate and temporal–perietal
cortex is involved in detecting and encoding novel
information (Halgren et al., 1998). Neuroimaging
results have confirmed the lesion and intracranial
evidence on the neuroanatomy of the novelty processing
system (Tulving et al., 1994; 1996; Stern et al., 1996;
McCarthy et al., 1997; Menon et al., 1997; Opitz et
al., 1999a,b; Yoshiura et al., 1999; Linden et al.,
1999; Downar et al., 2000; Clark et al., 2000; Kiehl et
al., in press). The lateral PFCx contribution is a key
component of this novelty network. For instance,
unlike posterior cortical and hippocampal activity,
PFCx novelty activation recorded with ERPs or
neuroimaging habituates to repeated exposures to novel
events and is modality independent (Knight, 1984;
Yamaguchi & Knight, 1991; Knight & Scabini, 1998;
Raichle et al., 1994; Petersson et al., 1999).
Importantly, lateral PFCx also appears to initiate the
novelty detection cascade prior to activation of other
brain regions as revealed by lesion-ERP studies. If
the novel event is sufficiently engaging, posterior
cortical and medial temporal regions are recruited for
further processing (Ahlo et al., 1994; Knight, 1996;
Novelty, of course, is an elusive concept
dependent on both the sensory parameters of an event
and the context in which it occurs. As an example, the
unexpected occurrence of a visual fractal would
typically engage the novelty system. Conversely, if one
would were presented with a stream of visual fractals
and suddenly a picture of an apple occurred this would
also drive the novelty system. In the first case the
visual complexity of the fractal drives the novelty
response. In the second situation the local context of
repeated fractals would be violated by the insertion of
a picture of an apple and this would also engage the
novelty network. Sensory parameters and local context
have powerful effects on electrophysiological and
behavioral response to novelty (Comerchero & Polich,
1998, 1999; Katayama & Polich, 1998) and this effect is
also dependent on lateral PFCx (Suwazono et al., 2000;
Neuroimaging findings in normal also support a
critical role in PFCx in responding to novel events and
solving new problems (See Duncan & Owen, 2000 for a
review). These neuropsychological, ERP and
neuroimaging findings support a central role of lateral
prefrontal cortex in the processing of novelty (Godfrey
& Rousseaux, 1997; Kimble et al., 1965). Single unit
data from monkeys has also supports a prefrontal bias
towards novelty (Rainer & Miller, 2000)
d. Response monitoring: Major advances have developed
in our understanding of the neural basis of
implementing neural control of behavioral output.
First, the discovery of an ERP response referred to as
the error-related negativity (ERN) has provided an
online measure of a subject’s performance monitoring
(Gehring et al., 1993). Second, neuroimaging data has
implicated a prefrontal-cingulate network in error
response monitoring and correction (McDonald et al.,
2000; Kiehl et al., 2000). Finally, lesion-ERP
evidence obtained from patients with lateral PFCx
damage supports the notion that PFCx controls cingulate
related error activity (Gehring & Knight, 2000). Cohen
and colleagues have suggested that the role of PFCx in
response monitoring is to provide a stable
representation of the task at hand (Carter et al.,
1999; Cohen et al., 2000). This permits better
suppression of distracting information lessening the
chance of an error. These authors suggest that the
cingulate ERN and fMRI blood flow response to errors is
a manifestation of conflict detection by the anterior
cingulate (areas 24 and 32). Thus, if the
representation of the task is weakened by PFCx damage,
conflict increases on all trials and an ERN is
generated to all stimuli. This view places the PFCx in
an executive position regarding anterior cingulate
function. An alternative model consistent with the
accrued data posits that the activity reflected in the
ERN represents an affective or motivational signal. In
this view, the cingulate signal as measured by the ERN
would serve an alerting function that mobilizes
affective systems, rather than immediate corrective
action, perhaps via cingulate connections with the
amygdala and brainstem autonomic nuclei. This
conception of the ERN would be consistent with
dissociations between ERN activity and compensatory
behavior and with reports of medial frontal ERN-like
activity in response to stimuli with negative hedonic
significance (Vidal et al., 2000; Falkenstein et al.,
Case Report, the Lateral Prefrontal Syndrome: Patient
A case study exemplifies the effects of lateral
prefrontal lesions. W.R., a 31-year-old lawyer,
presented to the Neurology clinic with family concern
over his lack of interest in important life events.
When queried as to why he was in the clinic, the
patient stated that he had "lost his ego". His
difficulties began four years previously in 1978 when
he had a tonic-clonic seizure after staying up all
night and drinking large amounts of coffee while
studying for midterm exams in his final year of law
school. An extensive neurological evaluation conducted
at that time at the NIH including EEG, CT scan, and PET
scan were all unremarkable. The diagnosis of
generalized seizure disorder exacerbated by sleep
deprivation was given and he was placed on dilantin.
W.R. graduated from law school but did not enter a
practice because he couldn't decide where to take the
bar exam. Over the next year he worked as a tennis
instructor in Florida. He then broke off a 2-year
relationship with a woman and moved to California to
live near his brother who was also a lawyer. His
brother reported that he was indecisive, procrastinated
in carrying out planned activities and that he was
becoming progressively isolated from family and
friends. The family attributed these problems to a
"mid-life crisis". Four months prior to neurological
consultation W.R.'s mother died. At the funeral and
during the time surrounding his mother’s death the
family noted that he expressed no grief regarding his
mother's death. The family decided to have the patient
re-evaluated. W.R. was pleasant but somewhat
indifferent to the situation. General neurological
exam was unremarkable. A mild snout reflex was
present. W.R. made both perseverative and random
errors on the Luria hand-sequencing task and was easily
distracted during the examination. His free recall was
two out of three words at a five-minute delay. He was
able to recall the third word with a semantic cue. On
questioning about his mother's death W.R. confirmed
that he did not feel any strong emotions, either about
his mother's death or about his current problem. The
patient's brother mentioned that W.R. "had never lost
it" emotionally during the week after his mother’s
death, at which point W.R. immediately interjected "and
I'm not trying not to lose it." Regarding his mother's
death, he stated "I don't feel grief, I don't know if
that's bad or good." These statements were emphatic,
but expressed in a somewhat jocular fashion
(witzelsucht). W.R. was asked about changes in his
personality. He struggled for some minutes to describe
changes he had noticed, but did not manage to identify
any. He stated "Being inside, I can't see it as
clear." He was distractible and perseverative,
frequently reverting to a prior discussion of tennis,
and repeating phrases such as "yellow comes to mind" in
response to queries of his memory. When asked about
either the past or the future, his responses were
schematic and stereotyped. He lacked any plans for the
future, initiated no future oriented actions and stated
"It didn't matter that much, it never bothered me" that
he never began to practice law. A CT scan revealed a
left lateral prefrontal glioblastoma, which had grown
through the corpus callosum into the lateral right
frontal lobe. After discussion of the serious nature
of the diagnosis, W.R. remained indifferent. The
family were distressed by the gravity of the situation
and showed appropriate anxiety and sadness.
Interestingly, they noted that their sadness was
Discussion of Patient W.R.: W.R. remained a pleasant
and articulate individual despite of his advanced
frontal tumor. However, he was unable to carry out the
activities to make him a fully functioning member of
society. His behavior was completely constrained by
his current circumstances. His jocularity was a
reaction to the social situation of the moment, and was
not influenced by the larger context of his recent
diagnosis. He appeared to have difficulty with
explicit memory and source monitoring, with little
confidence in his answers to memory queries,
complicated by frequent intrusions from internal mental
representations. Thus, metamemory was impaired and he
was unable to sustain working memory processes. He was
distractible and was unable to sustain normal working
memory. Perseverative errors were common in both the
motor and cognitive domain. A prominent aspect of his
behavior was a complete absence of counterfactual
expressions. In particular, WR expressed no
counterfactual emotions, being completely unable to
construe any explanation for his current behavioral
state. He seemed unable to feel grief or regret, nor
was he bothered by their absence even though he was
aware of his brother's concern over his absence of
emotion. These observations suggest that damage in
lateral PFCx leads to deficits in reality monitoring, a
process that is essential for the normal planning and
decision-making functions necessary for normal human
behavior. Behavioral analysis of this case highlights
the role of lateral PFCx in virtually all aspects of
In the simplest formulation, lateral prefrontal cortex
may be viewed as the central executive for cognitive
control with orbitofrontal cortex serving as the
central executive for emotional and social control.
In contrast to lateral prefrontal damage, orbitofrontal
damage spares many cognitive skills but dramatically
affects all spheres of social behavior (Stone et al.,
1998; Bechera et al., 1998). The orbitofrontal patient
is frequently impulsive, hyperactive and lacking in
proper social skills despite showing intact cognitive
processing on a range of tasks typically impaired in
the lateral PFCx lesioned patient. In some cases the
behavioral syndrome is so severe that the term acquired
sociopathy has been used to describe the resultant
personality profile of the orbitofrontal patient.
However, unlike true sociopaths, orbitofrontal patients
typically feel remorse for their inappropriate
behavior. Primitive reflexes such as snout, suck,
rooting and grasp are not often observed. Severe
social and emotional dysfunction is typically observed
only in bilateral orbital disease as might be observed
after head trauma, orbital meningioma or certain
degenerative disorders such as frontal-temporal
dementia. Thus, there appears to be redundancy in both
lateral and orbital human PFCx with one intact PFCx
being able to sustain many aspects of either cognitive
and social function. Similar to recent advances in
segregating function of lateral PFCx into dorsal and
ventral divisions, progress has been made in
parcellation of orbital PFCx function. The
ventromedial portion of the orbital PFCx has associated
with the use of internal autonomic states in the
guidance of goal directed behavior. The ventromedial
portion of human orbital PFCx has also been proposed to
be involved in inhibitory processing of emotional
stimuli. The lateral portions of orbital PFCx have
been implicated in the rapid establishment of reward-
punishment associations ( Shimamura, 2000; See Rolls
this volume). Tests of social and cognitive skills
reveal a double dissociation between lateral and
orbital PFCx damage. Lateral PFCx damage impairs
working memory and attention capacity but spares theory
of mind. Conversely, orbital PFCx damage leaves
working memory intact but impairs theory of mind. There
is some suggestion of an important role of the polar
region, but this has not yet been conclusively
Disorders of emotional control and social
regulation are frequent accompaniments of acquired
neurological disease and are receiving increasing
attention in the clinical and research arena (Stuss &
Alexander, 2000). In the 1930’s, Kluver & Bucy
described prominent affective and visual processing
changes in monkeys with bilateral anterior temporal
ablations. During this same period, Papez described the
classic “Circle of Papez” or limbic brain in humans
encompassing the anterior cingulate, hippocampus,
septum and hypothalamus. However, the two most critical
components of the human emotional control network,
orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala, were not included in
the original concept proposed by Papez. A seminal
observation linking brain damage and personality
alteration can be traced to 1848 in Cavendish, Vermont.
A well-respected train company employee, Phineas Gage,
was working clearing rocks necessary for the laying
down of a new rail line. An unfortunate accident
propelled an iron tamping rod through his skull.
Remarkably, given that the rod weighed 13 pounds, was
over 3 feet long and antibiotics were not yet
discovered, Gage survived. However, marked changes in
his previous calm and organized personality ensued.
Gage became more labile and disinhibited in his
behavior and was noted to use profanity and make
irreverent statements. His acquaintances noted that
“Gage was no longer Gage”. His problems continued
unabated until he died of uncontrolled seizures 12
years later in San Francisco. Inspection of his skull
indicates that the tract of the bar injured bilateral
orbitofrontal cortex and the anterior portion of the
left temporal lobe. However, the role of orbitofrontal
cortex in social behavior was largely neglected until
The most common cause of orbitofrontal and
amygdala damage is closed head injury with about
100,000 people per year in the US alone experiencing a
closed head injury severe enough to damage these
critical brain injuries. Orbitofrontal and amygdala
damage in not limited to head trauma and can also be
observed in dementing disorders such as Pick’s disease
or fronto-temporal dementia which has been linked to
abnormalities in chromosome 17 in some cases. In
addition, tumors including meningiomas and gliomas can
affect these areas and infections such as herpes
simplex have a particular predilection for the limbic
brain. Patients with an acquired non-progressive
lesion such as that due to head trauma may return to a
high level of pre-injury cognitive function. However,
as predicted by the Gage case, patients with
orbitofrontal or amygdala damage are impaired to
varying degrees in emotional control, social
interaction and decision making involving interpersonal
choices and behaviors. Many patients are initially
diagnosed incorrectly with a personality disorder when
in fact they have damaged their emotional brain.
Neurological examination, other than for frequent
anosmia, is invariably normal if damage is restricted
to orbital PFCx and there was no significant axonal
shear at the initial time of injury. Frontal release
signs including snout, suck, grasp and rooting are
absent. Remarkably little is known about the neural
underpinnings of this severely compromised “social
Several investigators have provided neuropsychological
data implicating orbital/ ventral-medial PFCx in
emotional and social regulation (Tranel & Damasio,
1994; Bechera et al., 1994; 1997; 2000; Rolls et al.,
1994; Grafman et al., 1993; Shammi & Stuss, 1999; Stone
et al., 1998; Hartikainen et al., 2000). Disorders of
emotional control and social regulation due to orbital
PFCx dysfunction are frequent accompaniments of
psychiatric disease such as obsessive compulsive
disorder and drug abuse (London et al., 2000; Volkow &
Fowler, 2000) as well as acquired neurological disease
including head trauma, dementia and tumors. Thus, the
societal costs of orbital PFCx dysfunction are immense.
Developmental aspects of acquired orbital PFCx damage
in children and adolescents are even less well
understood than adult dysfunction (Price et al., 1990).
Patients with adult acquired orbital PFCx are aware of
their problems and know the actual rules of proper
social behavior despite failures to properly implement
them. Childhood acquired orbital PFCx damage may result
in a failure to both implement and learn the rules of
proper social discourse (Anderson et al., 1999).
Changes in emotional disposition are routinely observed
in patients who have suffered damage to the
orbitofrontal cortex. Damage to this brain region has
been associated with a variety of social-emotional
dysfunctions, including personality change, risk
taking, impulsivity, emotional outbursts and social
inappropriateness. Three theories have been proposed to
explain the disordered behavior subsequent to orbital
damage in humans. These include the somatic marker
hypothesis put forth by Damasio and colleagues (Bechera
et al., 1994; see Tranel this volume), impaired linking
or reward and punishment proposed by Rolls (Rolls et
al., 1994; Rolls this volume) and emotional
disinhibition accompanied by enhanced central nervous
system responsivity recently proposed by Rule,
Shimamura and Knight (Rule et al., 1999; Shimamura,
For example, Damasio and colleagues have shown
that patients with orbital PFCx lesions elicit
inappropriate emotional responses and abnormal galvanic
skin responses in a gambling task in which subjects
must inhibit high-risk gambles (Bechera et al., 1997).
These authors propose that damage in the ventromedial
PFCX impairs generation of a somatic state that can be
used as a guide to control behavior. This proposal is
supported by reduced anticipatory GSRs in patients with
ventromedial damage. In another study a group of
lateral PFCx and a group orbital PFCx patients were
studied in working memory and theory of mind (TOM)
tasks. TOM refers to a person’s ability to infer
another person’s or group of person’s internal mental
state. TOM is viewed as one of the highest forms of
social abilities. A double dissociation was observed.
Lateral PFCx patients had difficulties with working
memory but were not impaired on TOM tasks. Orbital
PFCx patients were normal on working memory tasks but
failed TOM tasks (Stone et al., 1998). This finding
has been replicated and extended to indicate some
potential importance of the right frontal region in
another cohort of lateral and orbital lesioned patients
(Stuss et al., 2001). Taken together, these findings
are consistent with the notion that this brain region
is intricately involved in the analysis, monitoring and
control of emotionally laden stimuli and social
Functional neuroimaging currently has had difficulty
with imaging orbital PFCx due to susceptibility
artifacts from nearby sinuses and a limited number of
studies have been published (i.e. Schoenbaum et al.,
1998; Nobre et al., 1999; Elliott et al., 2000).
Recent studies indicate success with neuroimaging of
orbital regions (Doherty et al., 2001; Rolls this
volume). As noted in the Neuropsychology section,
three theories have been proposed to explain the
disordered behavior subsequent to orbital PFCx damage
in humans and all are supported by physiological data.
The somatic marker hypothesis (Bechera et al., 2000)
proposes that ventromedial orbital PFCx or the right
sensory cortical damage impairs generation of an
appropriate somatic feeling needed to guide behavior
(Tranel, 1994; Bechera et al., 1997; see Tranel this
volume). The somatic marker hypothesis is supported by
a decreased galvanic skin response (GSR), a peripheral
autonomic measure of orienting, in orbital PFCx
patients. Another view proposes impairments in linking
of reward and punishment (Rolls et al., 1994) and finds
support from single unit, PET and some fMRI research
(Elliott et al., 2000; see Rolls this volume). A third
theory, the dynamic filtering hypothesis, has also been
proposed to explain some components of the orbital PFCx
behavioral syndrome (Rule et al., 1999; Shimamura,
2000). This theory posits that orbital PFCx patients
are unable to inhibit response to certain emotional and
social stimuli and is supported by enhanced ERP
measures of orienting to novel emotionally laden
stimuli in these patients. The enhanced central nervous
system response to emotional auditory and somatosensory
stimuli in orbital PFCx patients is in accord with the
disinhibited, impulsive behavior observed after orbital
PFCx damage in humans and monkeys (Butter et al.,
1969,1970; Roberts et al., 2000). Interestingly,
Macaque monkeys with orbitofrontal lesions fail to
habituate to novel auditory and visual stimuli (Butter
et al., 1970). Importantly, these findings suggest
regional specificity within the prefrontal cortex. ERPs
to novel emotional stimuli are disinhibited patients
with orbital PFCx lesions, whereas patients with
lateral PFCx lesions show decreased novelty responses
to these same stimuli (Knight & Scabini 1998). These
results indicate that orbital patients may have an
excessive central nervous response to irrelevant
stimuli. Direct connections from orbitofrontal cortex
to posterior parietal cortex (area 7A) have been
identified (Cavada et al., 2000). Orbitofrontal cortex
could be exerting inhibitory control over novelty
related activity in the temporal-parietal region via
these fibers. Loss of this control might contribute to
the disinhibited behavior so frequently observed in
these patients. Elements of all three notions of
orbital function are likely correct and a more complete
concept of orbital contributions to social and
emotional behavior is likely to emerge in the ensuing
Case Report, the Orbital Prefrontal Syndrome: Patient
A case study exemplifies the effects of orbital PFCx
lesions. Patient JL was seen in neurological
consultation on the inpatient psychiatric service in
1988. He was admitted to the Psychiatric service after
an altercation at an intersection where he got into a
cursing and shoving match with a driver who cut him of
as he was crossing a street. JL was a 42-year-old
accountant with a Masters degree at the time of
evaluation. He had been in excellent health until an
accident at a party 13 years prior where he fell off a
third floor balcony and sustained a severe coup injury
to his frontal lobe. CT scanning revealed extensive
bilateral damage to his orbital PFCx. Both the
ventromedial and lateral portions of the orbital PFCx
were destroyed. JL developed grand mal seizures after
the accident, which were well controlled with dilantin.
Clinical and experimental neuropsychological evaluation
revealed a double dissociation between cognitive and
social function. Lateral PFCx was spared and all test
of cognitive function related to lateral PFCx were
intact. For instance, J.L.’s IQ remained at a pre-
morbid level of 128. Clinical and experimental tests
of memory including measures of source and metamemory
were intact. Attention capacity and working memory were
excellent. In contrast to his excellent cognitive
performance, since the incident J.L. has had prominent
problems in emotional and social control. He has
gotten into numerous street altercations and has been
arrested several times. He is socially inappropriate
and notes) that he “comes on too strong” to women.
Woman laboratory personnel where patient J.L. has been
tested report that he is constantly coming on to them.
When queried further, he reports that he might ask a
woman to marry him after 1-2 days of knowing her. When
asked if he thought this behavior was appropriate,
patient J.L. responded no. Importantly patient J.L.
knows his behavior is inappropriate but is unable to
control it. J.L. is also unable to handle the
financial resources that accrued as a settlement for
his accident and required a conservator to manage his
affairs. During the interview patient J.L. often
laughed inappropriately. His neurological examination
was normal including testing of language, attention,
memory and perception. He admitted to obsessive
compulsive behaviors such as counting the numbers on
car license plates. Testing of working memory was
normal but J.L. failed Theory of Mind tests, which
require the ability of a person to infer another
Discussion of Patient J.L.: Patient J.L. manifests the
typical orbital PFCx syndrome of intact executive
control of cognitive processes and severely impaired
executive control of social and emotional behavior (See
Rolls’ and Tranel’s contributions this volume). His IQ
was superior and he scored well in all conventional
tests of attention, memory and language. Yet, he was
severely impaired in his everyday social behavior and
in making appropriate life decisions as in managing his
financial affairs. Remarkably, when queried about what
was the appropriate social or emotional response or the
proper decision regarding personal affairs, patient
J.L. was able to respond correctly. His problems
became evident when he had to implement on-line
behavior. Explication of this apparent paradox of
knowledge versus failure to implement such knowledge is
What will the future bring to our understanding of
prefrontal function? Given the vast expansion of
prefrontal cortex in humans, explication of the
function of this brain region appears to a fundamental
for a complete understanding human cognition in both
health and disease. Advances have been made in multiple
domains. Cognitive psychology has provided a welcome
addition to the classic neuropsychological approach and
new areas of behavioral theory and analysis have
enriched our understanding of PFC function. We believe
that the next decade will witness an even greater
implementation of sophisticated cognitive theory to
prefrontal research. Approaches drawn from the
discipline of social cognition and from the study of
behaviors such as decision making and reality
monitoring, are certain to provide a broader and
ecologically valid approach to understanding PFC
function. This fusion of theory and experiment will
provide important new insights into the role of orbital
frontal cortex in social behavior. This book has
provided different views on the nature of orbital
frontal function in humans. We expect that Frontal Lobe
2010 will provide a more integrated view of how this
vast expanse of prefrontal cortex enables the social
One area likely to receive increasing attention is
the contribution of PFC to the evaluation and
implementation of context in behavior. The notion of
context is broadly has been applied to seemingly
diverse areas including probability learning, social
regulation and novelty detection. For instance, in the
social domain, a behavior in one situation might be
very appropriate while the same behavior could be quite
counter productive in another situation. Humans are
able to fluidly draw on prior experience to set and
implement the appropriate context for the current
situation. Similarly, in the area of novelty processing
the effects of local context are extremely powerful.
For instance, the occurrence of a visual fractal in a
stream of common visual objects would elicit a powerful
novelty response to the fractal. However, the
occurrence of a common object in a stream of fractals
would also elicit a powerful novelty response. Research
on the role of PFC in application of context dependent
parameters to behavior may prove critical for
understanding the role of PFC in mental flexibility.
Single unit studies in monkeys has been crucial in
developing new models of PFC function. The classic
ideas of segregation of function have been challenged
by findings that PFC neurons are more plastic than
traditional views might suggest. The concept of rapid
learning and plasticity of PFC neurons is in accord
with the neurological literature revealing profound
alterations in mental flexibility in patients with PFC
damage. This single unit research dovetails nicely
with the explosion of insights drawn from fMRI
research. Novel insights into segregation versus
integration of function in subregions of prefrontal has
fueled the debate. We now enjoy a powerful interplay
between human and monkey research that heralds major
advances on the understanding of cognitive processes.
We certainly believe that Frontal Lobe 2010 will
provide a clearer answer to the question of segregation
versus overlapping of function in prefrontal cortex.
We predict that as in many scientific controversies,
the final answer will blend data drawn from both camps.
How these executive processes are implemented at a
neural level is perhaps the greatest challenge for a
true understanding of PFC function. We certainly hope
that Frontal Lobe 2010 will fill in the crucial gaps in
our understanding this central aspect of human
cognition. The notion that engagement of parallel
inhibition and excitation can be a useful construct for
understanding PFC function is receiving support from
single unit, lesion, ERP and functional neuroimaging
research. Advances in the fusion of these experimental
approaches may provide new insights into both the
temporal and spatial aspects of PFC dependent executive
control. Consideration of the neuropharmacology of PFC
function will be necessary for a complete understanding
of prefrontal function and we hope this volume has
focussed attention on this needed part of the frontal
The nature of the neural code both at the local
single unit level and the systems interaction level is,
of course, central to a complete picture of PFC
function. How do single units in a subregion of PFC
interact to produce the needed output signal to other
brain regions? Are neurons concerned with inhibition
intertwined with those involved in excitation? What is
the nature of the output signal from PFC to other
neural regions? Is it a coherent burst of neural
activity such as a gamma oscillation? These questions
are only beginning to be addressed but promise great
insights into how PFC implements executive control.
What else might be discussed at Frontal Lobe 2010?
Certainly we are in the middle of an explosion of new
methods to image the human brain and this exponential
progress is likely to continue. Fusion of
electrophysiological and functional magnetic resonance
methods promises new insights into the temporal-spatial
dynamics of human cognition. Optical imaging
techniques have developed which may improve temporal
and spatial resolution. Perhaps as important, optical
techniques can be used to image infants extending the
field of imaging to the gamut of human development. We
wouldn’t be surprised if Frontal Lobe 2010 yields novel
information on the development of the frontal lobe from
Finally, why bother with all this fuss about the
prefrontal cortex? Is it because scientists deserve to
study what fascinates them? Certainly that brings and
keeps many researchers to the frontal lobe table.
However, that is not the true reason we spend our time
studying prefrontal cortex. Rather, we know that this
brain region holds the key to understanding normal and
disordered cognition with profound implications for
“The frontal lobes have provided the tools to now study
how tools are actually devised in the human mind.”
Ackerly, S.S., & Benton, A.L. (1947). Report of a
case of bilateral frontal lobe defect. Research Publications - Association for Research in Nervous
Ahlo, K., Woods, D.L., Algazi, A., Knight, R.T., &
Naatanen, R. (1994). Lesions of frontal cortex
diminish the auditory mismatch negativity.
Electroencephalography and Clinical
Alain, C., Woods, D.L., & Knight, R.T. (1998). A
distributed cortical network for auditory sensory
memory in humans. Brain Research, 812, 23-37.
Alexander, G.E., Newman, J.D., & Symmes, D. (1976).
Convergence of prefrontal and acoustic inputs upon
neurons in the superior temporal gyrus of the
awake squirrel monkey. Brain Research, 116, 334-
Anderson, S.W., Bechera, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., &
Damasio, A.R. (1999). Impairments in social and
moral behavior related to early damage in human
prefrontal corex. Nature Neuroscience, 2(11),
Asaad, W.F., Rainer, G., & Miller, E.K. (1998).
Neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortex
during associative learning. Neuron, 21(6), 1399-
Baddeley, A., & Wilson, B. (1988). Frontal amnesia
and the dysexecutive syndrome. Brain and
Bahramali, H., Gordon, E., Lim, C.L., Li, W.,
Lagapoulus, J., Rennie, C., & Meares, R.A.
(1997). Evoked related potentials with and
without an orienting reflex. Neuroreport, 8,
Barcelo, F., & Knight, R.T. (In press). Both random
and perseverative errors underlie WCST deficits in
prefrontal patients. Neuropsychologia.
Barcelo, P., Suwazono, S., & Knight, R.T. (2000).
Prefrontal modulation of visual processing in
humans. Nature Neuroscience, 3(4), 399-403.
Barcelo, F., & Knight, R.T. (2000). Prefrontal
lesions alter context dependent value of novel
stimuli during visual attention. Society of
Barch, D.M., Braver, T.S., Sabb, F.W., & Noll, D.C.
(2000). Anterior cingulate and the monitoring of
response conflict: evidence from an fMRI study of
verb generation. Journal of Cognitive
Bartus, R.T., & Levere, T.E. (1977). Frontal
decortication in Rhesus monkeys: A test of the
interference hypothesis. Brain Research, 119,
Bechera, A., Damasio, H., Tranel D., & Anderson, S.W.
(1998). Dissociation of working memory from
decision making within the human prefrontal
cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(1), 428-37.
Bechera, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A.R. (2000).
Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal
cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 295-307.
Bechera, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A.R.
(1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing
the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293-
Bechera, A., Damasio, A.R., Damasio, H., & Anderson,
consequences floowing damage to human prefrontal
Benjamin, J., Li, L., Patterson, C., Greenberg, B.D.,
Murphy, D.L., & Hamer, D.H. (1996). Population
and familial association between the D4 dopamine
receptor gene and measures of novelty seeking.
Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L.E., Fissell, K., Carter,
C.S., & Cohen, J.D. (1999). Conflict monitoring
versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate
cortex. Nature, 402(6758), 179-81.
Brutkowski, S. (1965). Functions of prefrontal cortex
in animals. Physiolological Review, 45, 721-746.
Büchel, C., & Friston, K.J. (1997). Modulation of
connectivity in visual pathways by attention:
Cortical interactions evaluated with structural
equation modeling and fMRI. Cerebral Cortex, 7,
Butter, C.M., Snyder, D.R., & McDonald, J.A. (1970).
Effects of orbital frontal lesions on aversive and
aggressive behaviors in Rhesus monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology,
Butter, C.M., McDonald, A., & Snyder, D.R. (1969).
Orality, preference behavior, and reinforcement
value of nonfood object in monkeys with orbital
frontal lesions. Science, 164(3885), 1306-1307.
Carter, C.S., Botvinick, M.M., & Cohen, J.D. (1999).
The contribution of the anterior cingulate cortex
to executive processes in cognition. Reviews in
Cavada, C., Company, T., Tejedor, J., Cruz-Rizzolo,
R.J., & Reinoso-Suarez, F. (2000). The
anatomical connections of the Macaque monkey
orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 220-
Chao, L.L., & Knight, R.T. (1998). Contribution of
human prefrontal cortex to delay performance.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 167-177.
Chao, L.L., & Knight, R.T. (1995). Human prefrontal
lesions increase distractibility to irrelevant
sensory. NeuroReport, 6, 1605-1610.
Chawla, D., Rees, G., & Friston, K.J. (1999). The
physiological basis of attentional modulation in
extrastriate visual areas. Nature Neuroscience,
Clark, V.P., Fannon, S., Lai, S., Benson, R., & Bauer,
L. (2000). Responses to rare visual target and
distractor stimuli using fMRI. Journal of
Cohen, J.D., Braver, S.B., & O’Reilly, R.C. (1996). A
computational approach to prefrontal cortex,
cognitive control and schizophrenia: recent
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of
Cohen, J.D., Botvinivk, M., & Carter, C.S. (2000).
Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex: who’s in
control? Nature Neuroscience, 3(5), 421-423.
Comerchero, M.D., & Polich, J. (1998). P3a,
perceptual distinctiveness, and stimulus modality. Cognitive Brain Resarch, 7(1), 41-8.
Comerchero, M.D., & Polich, J. (1999). P3a and P3b
from typical auditory and visual stimuli.
Clinical Neurophysiology, 110(1), 24-30.
Corbetta, M. (1998). Frontoparietal cortical networks
for directing attention and the eye to visual
locations: Identical, independent, or overlapping
neural systems? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95, 831-838.
Courchesne, E., Hillyard, S.A., & Galambos, R. (1975).
Stimulus novelty, task relevance, and the visual
evoked potential in man. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 39, 131-143.
Courtney, S.M., Petit, L., Maisog, J.M., Ungerleider,
L.G., & Haxby, J.V. (1998). An area specialized
for spatial working memory in human frontal
Daffner, K.R., Mesulam, M.-M., Holcomb, P.J., Calvo,
V., Acar, D., Chabrerie, A., Kikinis, R., Jolesz,
F.A., Rentz, D.M., Scinto, L.F. (2000a).
Disruption of attention to novel events after
frontal lobe injury in humans. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 68(1), 18-
Daffner K.R., Mesulam, M.M., Scinto, L.F., Acar, D.,
Calvo, V., Faust, R., Chabrerie, A., Kennedy, B.,
& Holcomb, P. (2000b). The central role of the
prefrontal cortex in directing attention to novel
Daffner, K.R., Mesulam, M.-M., Scinto, L.F., Calvo, V.,
West, W.C., & Holcomb, P. (2000). The influence
of stimulus deviance on electrophysiologic and
behavioral response to novel events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(3), 393-406.
Damasio, A.S. (1985). The frontal lobes. In K.M.
Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical Neuropsychology, 2nd Edition (pp.339-374). New
Desimome, R. (1998). Visual attention mediatd by
biased competition in extrastriate cortex.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 353, 1245-
D'Esposito, M., Detre, J.A., Alsop, D.C., Shin, R.K.,
Atlas, S., & Grossman, M. (1995). The neural
basis of the central executive system of working
D'Esposito, M., & Postle, B.R. (1999a). The
D'Esposito, M., Postle, B.R., Ballard, D., & Lease, J.
(1999b). Maintenance versus manipulation of
information held in working memory: an event-
related fMRI study. Brain & Cognition, 41, 66-86.
D'Esposito, M., Postle, B.R., Jonides, J., & Smith,
E.E. (1999c). The neural substrate and temporal
dynamics of interference effects in working memory
as revealed by event-related functional MRI.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Science,
Dias, R., Robbins, T.W., & Roberts, A.C. (1997).
Dissociable forms of inhibitory control within
prefrontal cortex with an analog of the Wisconsin
Card Sort Test: restriction to novel situations
and independence from "on-line" processing.
Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 9285-97.
Downar, J., Crawley, A.P., Mikulis, D.J., & Davis, K.D.
(2000). A multimodal cortical network for the
detection of changes in the sensory environment. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3), 277-283.
Dronkers, N.F., Redfern, B.B., & Knight, R.T. (2000).
The neural architecture of language disorders. In
M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The New Cognitive Neurosciences (pp.949-958). MIT Press.
Duncan, J., & Owen, A.M. (2000). Common regions of
the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse
cognitive demands. Trends in Neuroscience, 10,
Ebstein, R.P., Novic, O., Umansky, R., Prie, B., Osher,
V., Blaine, D., Bennett, E.R., Nemanov, L., Katz,
M., & Belmaker, R.H. (1996). Dopamine D4
receptor (D4DR) exon III polymorphism associated
with the human personality trait of novelty
seeking. Nature Genetics, 12, 78-80.
Edinger, H.M., Siegel, A., & Troiano, R. (1975).
Effect of stimulation of prefrontal cortex and
amygdala on diencephalic neurons. Brain Research,
Escera, C., Alho, K., Winkler, I., & Naatanen, R.
(1998). Neural mechanisms of involuntary
attention to acoustic novelty and change. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(5), 590-604.
Elliott, R., Dolan, R.J., & Frith, C.D. (2000).
Dissociable functions in the medial and lateral
orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from human
neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 308-
Eslinger, P.J., & Damasio, A.R. (1985). Severe
disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral
frontal lobe ablation: Patient EVR. Neurology,
Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., Christ, S., &
Hiohnsbein, J. (2000). ERP components on
reaction errors and their functional significance:
a tutorial. Biological Psychology, 51(2-3), 87-
Funahashi, S., Bruce, C.J., & Goldman-Rakic, P.S.
(1993). Dorsolateral prefrontal lesions and
oculomotor delayed-response performance: Evidence
for mnemonic “scotomas”. Journal of Neuroscience,
Fuster, J.M. (1989). The prefrontal cortex: anatomy, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe.Second edition. New York: Raven Press.
Fuster, J.M., Brodner, M., & Kroger, J.K. (2000).
Cross-modal and cross-temporal associations in
neurons of frontal cortex. Nature, 405, 347-351.
Gehring, W.J., & Knight, R.T. (2000). Prefrontal-
cingulate interactions in action monitoring.
Nature Neuroscience, 3(5), 516-520.
Gehring, W.J., Goss, B., Coles, M.G.H., Meyer, D.E., &
Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error
detection and compensation. Psychological
Godfrey, O., & Rousseaux, M. (1997). Novel decision
making in patients with prefrontal or posterior
brain damage. Neurology, 49, 695-701.
Goldberg, E., Podell, K., & Lovell, M. (1994).
Lateralization of frontal lobe functions and
cognitive novelty. J Neuropsychiatry and Clin
Goldstein, K. (1944). Mental changes due to frontal
lobe damage. Journal of Psychology, 17, 187-208.
Grady, C.L., Van Meter, J.W., Maisog, J.M., Pietrini,
P., Krasuski, J., & Rauschecker, J.P. (1997).
Attention-related modulation of activity in
Grafman, J., Jonas, B., & Salazar, A. (1990).
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance based on
location and size of neuroanatomical lesion in
Vietnam veterans with penetrating head injury.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 71, 1120-1122.
Grafman, J., Vance, S.C., Weingartner, H., Slazar,
A.M., & Amin, D. (1993). The effects of
lateralized frontal lesions on mood regulation.
Guillery, R.W., Feig, S.L., & Lozsadi, D.A. (1998).
Paying attention to the thalamic reticular
nucleus. Trends in Neuroscience, 21, 28-32.
Guitton, D., Buchtel, H.A., & Douglas, R.M. (1985).
Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in
suppressing reflexive glances and in generating
goal-directed saccades. Experimental Brain
Halgren, E., Marinkovic, K., & Chauvel, P. (1998).
Generators of the late cognitive potential in
Electroencephalography and Clinical
Harrington, D.L., Haaland, K.Y., & Knight, R.T.
(1998). Cortical networks underlying mechanisms
of time perception. Journal of Neuroscience,
Hartikainen, K.M., Ogawa, K.H., Soltani, M., Pepitone,
M., & Knight, R.T. (2000). Altered emotional
influence on visual attention subsequent to
orbitofrontal damage in humans. Society for
Hebb, D.O., & Penfield, W. (1940). Human behavior
after extensive bilateral removals from the
frontal lobes. Arch Neurol Psych, 4, 421-438.
Heinze, H.J., Mangun, G.R., Burchert, W., Hinrichs, H.,
Scholz, M., Munte, T.F., Gos, A., Scherg, M.,
Johannes, S., Hundeshagen, H., Gazzaniga, M.S., &
Hillyard, S.A. (1994). Combined spatial and
temporal imaging of brain activity during visual
selective attention in humans. Nature, 372, 543-
Hillyard, S.A., R.F. Hink, U.L. Schwent, & T.W. Picton.
(1973). Electrical signs of selective attention
in the human brain. Science, 182, 177-180.
Hillyard, S.A., & Anllo-Vento, L. (1998). Event-
related brain potentials in the study of visual
selective attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95, 781-787.
Hopfinger, J.P., Buonocore, M.H., & Mangun, G.R.
(2000). The neural mechanisms of top-down
attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3),
Jacobsen, C.F. (1935). Functions of frontal
association areas in primates. Arch Neurol
Janowsky, J.S., Shimamura, A.P., & Squire, L.R.
(1989a). Memory and metamemory: Comparisons
between patients with frontal lobe lesions and
amnesic patients. Psychobiology, 17, 3-11.
Janowsky, J.S., Shimamura, A.P., & Squire, L.R.
(1989b). Source memory impairment in patients
with frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 27,
Johnson, M.K., & Raye, C.L. (1981). Reality
monitoring. Psychological Review, 88(1), 67-85.
Jonides, J., Smith, E.E., Koeppe, R.A., Awh, E.,
Minoshima, S., & Mintun, M.A. (1993). Spatial
working memory in humans as revealed by PET.
Jonides, J., Smith, E.E., Marshuetz, C., Koeppe, R.A.,
& Reuter-Lorenz, P.A. (1998). Inhibition of
activation. Proceedings of the National Academy
Kahneman, D., & Miller, D.T. (1986). Norm theory:
Psychological Review, 93(2), 136-153.
Kahneman, D., Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective
probability: A judgement of representativeness.
Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430-454.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The simulation
heuristic. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A.
Tversky (Eds.), Judgement under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases (pp.201-208). New York:
Kahneman, D., & Varey, C.A. (1990). Propensities and
counterfactuals: The loser that almost won.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Karis, D., Fabiani, M., & Donchin, E. (1984). "P300"
and memory: individual differences in the Von
Restorff effect. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 177-
Kastner, S., Pinsk, M.A., de Weerd, P., Desimone, R., &
Ungerleider, L.G. (1999). Increased activity in
human visual cortex during directed attention in
the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron, 22,
Katayama, J., & Polich, J. (1998). Stimulus context
determines P3a and P3b. Psychophysiology, 35(1),
Kertesz, A., & Dobrolowski, S. (1981). Right-
hemisphere deficits, lesion size and location.
Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 3, 283-299.
Kiehl, K.A., Laurens, K.R., Duty, T.L., Forster, B.B.,
& Liddle, P.F. (In press). An event-related fMRI
study of a visual oddball task, Psychophysiology.
Kiehl, K.A., Liddle, P.F., & Hopfinger, J.B. (2000).
Error processing and the anterior cingulate: an
event-related fMRI study. Psychophysiology,
Kimble, D.P., Bagshaw, M.H., & Pribram, K.H. (1965).
habituation after selective partial ablations of
the cingulate and frontal cortex.
Knight, R.T. (1996). Contribution of human
hippocampal region to novelty detection. Nature,
Knight, R.T., Staines, W.R., Swick, D., & Chao, L.L.
(1998). Prefrontal cortex regulates inhibition
and excitation in distributed neural networks.
Knight, R.T. (1997). Distributed cortical network for
visual attention. Journal of Cognitive
Knight, R.T. (1991). Evoked potential studies of
attention capacity in human frontal lobe lesions.
In H. Levin, H. Eisenberg, & F. Benton (Eds.),
Frontal Lobe Function and Dysfunction (pp.139-
Knight, R.T., & Scabini, D. (1998). Anatomic bases of
event-related potentials and their relationship to
novelty detection in humans. Journal of Clinical
Knight, R.T. (1984). Decreased response to novel
stimuli after prefrontal lesions in man.
Electroencephalography and Clinical
Knight, R.T., Scabini, D., & Woods, D.L. (1989a).
Prefrontal Cortex gating of auditory transmission
in humans. Brain Research, 504, 338-342.
Knight, R.T., Scabini, D., Woods, D.L., & Clayworth,
C.C. (1989b). Contribution of the temporal-
parietal junction to the auditory P3. Brain
Knight, R.T., Hillyard, S.A., Woods, D.L., & Neville,
H.J. (1981). The effects of frontal cortex
lesions on event-related potentials during
Electroencephalography and Clinical
Knight, R.T., & Grabowecky, M. (2000). Prefrontal
ortex, time and consciousness. In M. Gazzaniga
(Ed.), The New Cognitive Neurosciences (pp.1319-
Konishi, S., Naakjima, K., Uchida, I., Kaneyama, M.,
Nakahara, K., Sekihara, K., & Miyashita, Y.
(1998). Transient activation of inferior
prefrontal cortex during cognitive set shifting.
Nature Neuroscience, 1(1), 80-84.
Lhermitte, F., Pillon, B., & Serdaru, M. (1986).
Human anatomy and the frontal lobes. Part I:
neuropsychological study of 75 patients. Annals
Lhermitte, F. (1986). Human autonomy and the frontal
lobes. Part II: patient behavior in complex and
social situations: The "environmental dependency
syndrome". Annals of Neurology, 19, 335-343.
Linden, D.E.J., Prvulovic, D., Formisano, E.,
Vollinger, M., Zanella, F.E., Goebel, R., &
Dierks, T. (1999). The functional neuroanatomy
of target detection: and fMRI study of visual and
auditory oddball tasks. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 815-
London, E.D., Ernst, M., Grant, S., Bonson, K., &
Weinstein, A. (2000). Orbitofrontal cortex and
human drug abuse: functional imaging. Cerebral
Luck, S.J., Chelazzi, L., Hillyard, S.A., & Desimone,
selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of
macaque visual cortex. Journal of
Luria, A.R. (1966). Higher Cortical Functions in Man.
Luu, P., Flaisch, T., & Tucker, D.M. (2000). Medial
frontal cortex in action monitoring. Journal of
Malmo, R.R. (1942). Interference factors in delayed
response in monkeys after removal of frontal
lobes. Journal of Neurophysiology, 5, 295-308.
Mangels, J., Gershberg, F.B., Shimamura, A., & Knight,
R.T. (1996). Impaired retrieval from remote
memory in patients with frontal lobe damage.
Mangun, G.R. (1995). Neural mechanisms of visual
selective attention. Psychophysiology, 32, 4-18.
Martinez, A., Anllo-Vento, L., Sereno, M.I., Frank,
L.R., Buxton, R.B., Dubowitz, D.J., Wong E.C.,
Hinrichs, H., Heinze, H.J., & Hillyard, S.A.
(1999). Involvement of striate and extrastriate
visual cortical areas in spatial attention.
Nature Neuroscience, 2(4), 364-9.
McAndrews, M.P., & Milner, B. (1991). The frontal
Neuropsychologia, 29(9), 849-859.
McCarthy, G., Luby, M., Gore, J., & Goldman-Rakic, P.S.
(1997). Infrequent events transiently activate
human prefrontal and parietal cortex as measured
by functional MRI. Journal of Neurophysiology,
McDonald, A.W., Cohen, J.D., Stenger, V.A., & Carter,
C.S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the
dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate
cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288, 1835-
McIntosh, A.R., Grady, C.L., Ungerleider, L.G., Haxby,
J.V., Rapoport, S.I., & Horwitz, B. (1994).
Network analysis of cortical visual pathways
mapped with PET. Journal of Neuroscience, 14,
McIntosh, A.R., Rajah, M.M., & Lobaugh, N.J. (1999).
Interactin of prefrontal coretex in relation to
awareness in sensory learning. Science, 284,
Menon, K., Ford, J.M., Lim, K.O., Glover, G.H., &
Pfefferbaum, A. (1997). A combined event-related
fMRI and EEG evidence for temporal-parietal
activation during target detection. NeuroReport,
Mesulam, M.-M. (1981). A cortical nerwork for
directed attention and unilateral neglect. Annals
Mesualam, M.-M. (1998). From sensation to cognition.
Miller, E.K. (1999). The prefrontal cortex: complex
neural properties for complex behavior. Neuron,
Milner, B. (1971). Interhemispheric differences in
the localization of psychological processes in
man. British Medical Bulletin, 27, 272-277.
Milner, B., & Petrides, M. (1984). Behavioural
effects of frontal-lobe lesions in man. Trends in
Milner, B., Petrides M., & Smith, M.L. (1985).
Frontal lobes and the temporal organization of
memory. Human Neurobiology, 4, 137-142.
Moscovich, M. (1989). Confabulation and the frontal
systems: Strategic versus associative retrieval in
neuropsychological theories of memory. In H.L.
Roedigger III & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of Memory and Consciousness: Essays in Honour of Endel Tulving (pp.133-160). Hillsdale, N.J.:
Muller, N.G., & Knight, R.T. (In press). Contribution
of subregions of the prefrontal cortex to working
memory. Evidence from brain lesions in humans.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
Nielsen-Bohlman, L., & Knight, R.T. (1999).
Prefrontal cortical involvement in visual working
memory. Cognitive Brain Research, 8, 299-310.
Nobre, A.C., Coull, J.T., Frith, C.D., & Mesualm, M.-M.
(1999). Orbitofrontal cortex is activated during
breaches of expectation in tasks of visual
attention. Nature Neuroscience, 2(1), 11-12.
Opitz, B., Mecklinger, A., Friederici, A.D., & von
Cramon, D.Y. (1999). The functional neuroanatomy
of novelty processing: integrating ERP and fMRI
results. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 379-91.
Opitz, B., Meclinger, A., Vob Cramon, D.Y., & Krugel,
F. (1999). Combining electrophysiological and
hemodynamic measures of the auditory oddball.
Owen, A.M., Stern, C.E., Look, R.B., Tracey, I., Rosen,
B.R., & Petrides, M. (1998). Functional
organization of spatial and nonspatial working
memory processing within the human lateral frontal
cortex. Proceedings National Academy Sciences,
Peterson, B.S., Skudlarski, P., Gatenby, J.C., Zhang,
Int J Colorectal Dis (2011) 26:1127–1134DOI 10.1007/s00384-011-1213-9β-catenin and Her2/neu expression in rectal cancer:association with histomorphological response to neoadjuvanttherapy and prognosisUta Drebber & Martin Madeja & Margarete Odenthal & Inga Wedemeyer &Stefan P. Mönig & Jan Brabender & Elfriede Bollschweiler & Arnulf H. Hölscher &Paul M. Schneid
Proposition de sujet de thèse 2013 (A remplir par les équipes d'accueil et à retourner à Isabelle HAMMAD : [email protected] MASTER : Océanographie - Option Biologie et Écologie Marine (OSU Pythéas) Ordre de priorité de la proposition(1) : Candidat(e) (1) Nom - Prénom : Date de naissance : Cursus de second cycle (origine, années, mention) : Mention et classement au Ma